• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Measuring Language Variation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Aktie "Measuring Language Variation"

Copied!
20
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Measuring Language Variation

Therese Leinonen

25 June 2008

(2)

Overview

• Backgound

• Measuring dialect variation with Levenshtein distance

• The phonetic puzzle

• Levenshtein distance and perceptual distance

• SweDia2000

• Measuring dialect variation acoustically

• Visualizing results: Multidimensional scaling

• Future work

(3)

Background

• dialectometry = measuring dialect. Term invented buy Jean Séguy.

• aim: find dialect borders and explore dialect continua

• method: find a measure for measuring linguistic distance between dialects

(4)

Levenshtein distance

• edit distance, calculates the cost of changing one string to another

• applied for comparison of Irish dialects by Kessler 1995

• later applied to American English, Bantu languages, Bulgarian, Chinese, Dutch, German, Norwegian, Sardinian

• example Lyngby [Pe:ni] vs. Helsinki [e:nIA] ’agreed’

Lyngby Pe:ni remove P 1 e:ni substitute i by I 1 e:nI insert A 1 Helsinki e:nIA

3

(5)

Levenshtein distance

Length normalization

1 2 3 4 5

Lungby P e: n i

Helsinki e: n I A

del sub ins

non-normalized distance: 3

normalized distance: 3/5 = 0.6 or 60 %

(6)

Phonetic Puzzle

• theorem: given segment distances, Levenshtein algorithm finds optimal alignment

• what are good segment distances?

• various feature systems: Vieregge-Cucchiarini, Almeida-Braun

• "acoustic" distance

• stochastic learning procedure (Pair Hmms)

• very limited improvement over binary segmental table

(7)

Phonetic Puzzle

Why is detailed phonetic information not helping?

• hypothesis 1: transcriptions are phonetically unreliable

• hypothesis 2: previous attempts were too ambitious, trying to characterize all distinctions

• hypothesis 3: we are past the size where fine discrimination matters

• others?

(8)

Predicting intelligibility and perceived linguistic distance (Beijering, Gooskens and Heeringa 2008)

Research questions:

• How well can Levenshtein distance predict perceptive distance and intel- legibility?

• How well can normalized Levenshtein distance predict perceptive distance in comparison to non-normalized Levenshtein distace?

Data:

• recordings of The North Wind and the Sun in 18 Scandinavian varieties

• phonetic transcriptions of cognates (on average 98 words)

(9)

Predicting intelligibility and perceived linguistic distance (Beijering et al. 2008)

Perceptual distance:

• listeners: 3 groups 15-19-year-olds from Copenhagen

• stimulus data: the whole recording of the fable in 6 varieties

• task: judge distance to Standard Danish on a scale from 1 to 10

Intellegibility:

• listeners: 18 groups 15-19-year-olds from Copenhagen

• stimulus data: 6 sentences in 6 varieties

• task: translate into Standard Danish

(10)

Predicting intelligibility and perceived linguistic distance (Beijering et al. 2008)

Correlation with Levenshtein distance:

normalized non-normalized Perceptual distance 0.52 0.62

Intellegibility -0.86 -0.79

Differences between normalized and non-normalized Levenshtein distances are not significant.

Conclusion: Levenshtein distance a better predictor of intelligibility than of perceived linguistic distances

(11)

Swedish vowel data

• SweDia2000: project carried out by the univer- sities of Lund, Stockholm and Umeå 1998-2001 (Bruce, Elert, Engstrand and Eriksson 1999)

• 105 sites in Sweden and Swedish-speaking Fin- land

• 12 speakers from each site: 3 elderly women, 3 elderly men, 3 young women, 3 young men

• vowels elicited with existing one-syllable words with the target vowel in a coronal consonant con- text

• 19 words of which the vowels cover the standard Swedish vowel space

(12)

Acoustic method

• principal component analysis (PCA) on bandfiltered spectra (Jacobi, Pols and Stroop 2005, Pols, Tromp and Plomp 1973)

• vowel spectra filtered up to 18 Bark

• PCA built on 4 anchor vowels ([i], [æ], [a] and [u]) of equally many men and women from every site (in total 300 speakers from 83 sites)

• two first principal components (85.6 % of total variance explained) used as acoustic measure of vowel quality

• creaky voice is a problem for the method: F0 controll

(13)

Factor loadings

(14)

Factor scores

(15)

Dialect distances

• linguistic distances measured for all pair of sites: Euclidean distances of pc1 and pc2 of all words (averages per site)

s n

P

i=1

(pi − qi)2

• distances analyzed with multidimensional scaling (MDS): vizualisation of dis- tances in a low dimensional space

• visualizing three dimensions with RGB-colours gives maps that can show a dialect continuum (Heeringa 2004)

(16)

MDS: dimensions 1 and 2

(17)

MDS: dimensions 3, 4 and 5

(18)
(19)

Future work

• work on the acoustic method (rotation)

• include more measuring points within a segment (diphthongization)

• extracting underlying linguistic structure (PCA)

(20)

References

Beijering, K., Gooskens, C. and Heeringa, W.(2008), Predicting intelligibility and perceived linguistic distance by means of the Levenshtein algorithm, LIN-bundel. in press.

Bruce, G., Elert, C.-C., Engstrand, O. and Eriksson, A.(1999), Phonetics and phonology of the Swedish dialects:

a project presentation and a database demonstrator, Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 99), San Francisco.

Heeringa, W.(2004), Measuring Dialect Pronunciation Differences using Levenshtein Distance, PhD thesis, Rijk- suniversiteit Groningen, Groningen.

Jacobi, I., Pols, L. C. W. and Stroop, J.(2005), Polder Dutch: Aspects of the /ei/-lowering in Standard Dutch, Interspeech’05, Lisboa, pp. 2877–2880.

Pols, L. C. W., Tromp, H. R. C. and Plomp, R.(1973), Frequency analysis of Dutch vowels from 50 male speakers, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America53, 1093–1101.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

‘ipsi’ indicates regions of interest in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the stenosis while ‘contra’ refers to the regions of interest in the hemisphere contralateral to the

In Chapter 3 we prove an analogue of Theorem 1.1.1 for totally skewed stable L´ evy processes.. In Chapter 4 we prove a result on the almost sure limiting behavior of

Given a context structure, an al- gorithm for estimating context-dependent probabilities of elementary editing operations from a given finite training corpus is designed..

Es wird keine Haftung übernommen für Schäden durch die Verwendung von Informationen aus diesem Online-Angebot oder durch das Fehlen von Informationen.. Dies gilt auch für

Under assumptions C %ym(.. The generalization of the discussed results to the ST0 problems with constraints in expectations can be done directly under

Actio: Herr Botschafter, wir Haben d/e ungeheuer profeske Situation, dass d/e k/e/ne Schweiz, d/e fofa/ au/ das Aus/and angewiesen ist we// es keinen eigenen Höhsfofthaf, eine

• The straight line has a systematic error and does not reflect the true relation between speed and braking distance.. From physics, we know that a parabola is

In such a setting, Cook’s distance does not work reliably as a measure for influential data points as leaving out a single observation does not change much (and Cook’s distance is