• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Flexible Tenacity in Goal Pursuit

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Flexible Tenacity in Goal Pursuit"

Copied!
17
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Flexible Tenacity in Goal Pursuit

Peter M. Gollwitzer Elizabeth J. Parks-Stamm Alexander Jaudas PaschaI Sheeran

Goal-directed behavior possesses various ob- servable features (Gollwitzer & Moskowitz, 1996). First and foremost, it is characterizedby persistent striving until the goal is reached.

Second, goal-directedness expresses itself in energization when situations or means that can be used to reach the goal are encountered. And third,goal-directed organisms show appropri- ateness: If one route to goal attainment is blocked, another course of action to the same goal is taken. Alternatively, if the goal changes, the goal-directed individual readily adapts to these changes by performing different actions.

Ir is the aspect of appropriateness that is scrutinized in the present chapter. We assume that goal pursuits, conscious as weIl as non- conscious (Gollwitzer & Bargh, 2005), possess much flexibility. But we wanttocarry this issue one step further. We raise the question of whether very tenacious goal pursuits do still possess the feature of appropriateness, or whether such determined goal pursuits are characterized by rigidity. The kind of height-

ened tenacity we analyze is not that of in- creased energization, which is experienced in the face of difficulty (Wright, 1996). Rather, we focus on the tenacity that originates from plan- ning out an intended goal pursuit in advanee.

As research on implementation intentions (i.e., if-then plans) has observed (see summaries by Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer, Bayer, &

MeCuIloch, 2005; Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006), individuals who furnish their goals with implementation intentions achieve higher goal attainment rates than individuals who act on the basis of mere goals (or goal intentions). The question we want to answer first in the present chapter is therefore the following: Are the ben- efits of forming implementation intentions as- sociated with costs in terms of reduced flexibil- ity (appropriateness)? Gnee we have found an answer to this first question, we turn in the see- ond part of the chapter to the possibility of us- ing implementation intentions to achieve flexi- ble tenacity. The seeond question raised is thus quite different: Can forming an implementa- 325

First publ. in: Handbook of motivation science / ed. by James Y. Shah ... New York: Guilford Pr., 2007, pp. 325-341

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://www.ub.uni-konstanz.de/kops/volltexte/2008/6373/

URN: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-63734

(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
(10)
(11)
(12)
(13)
(14)
(15)
(16)
(17)

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

First, studies that measured goal intentions or self- efficacy both before and after respective implementation intent.ion ind.ucti~ns fou~d .no evidence that plan formation

Consistent with research on goal priming, individuals in the present study should behave in line with their goals and thus be more combative in the combative goal condition as

If people simply dream about a positive future, or only dwell on the negative reality, it is not guaranteed that high-feasibility beliefs are indeed translated into strong

[r]

Objective: Research on the strength model of self-regulation is burgeoning, but little empirical work has focused on the link between distinct types of daily goal pursuit and

We hypothesized that pairs of loss-frame negotiators with a prosocial goal intention should manage to reduce the resistance to concession making arising from the

As implementation intentions traditionally refer to the individual, my model thus proposes a new type of plan that refers to the group, collective imple- mentation intentions (cII),

Accordingly, we had a no- goal control condition and a mere goal intention condi- tion in which tennis players the day before a critical match were assigned the following goal: “I