• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

openlib

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "openlib"

Copied!
1
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Discriminating goal-directed from nongoal-directed movements and its potential impact for BCI control

J. Pereira

1

, P. Ofner

1

, A. Schwarz

1

, G. R. Müller-Putz

1*

1Institute of Neural Engineering, Graz University of Technology, Graz, Austria

*Stremayrgasse 16/IV, 8010 Graz, Austria. E-mail: gernot.mueller@tugraz.at

Introduction: Differences in the electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings between the execution of goal- directed and nongoal-directed movements have been recently shown in [1]. Such differences can be of interest for brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) control, when combined with information on the kinematic level (e.g.

velocity decoding), since this combination mirrors the hierarchic way one plans a movement. In this study, we show that the time-domain differences between these movements are discriminable in a single-trial classification.

Material, Methods and Results: Ten healthy, right-handed subjects participated in the experiment. Subjects were presented a small red ball on the monitor (Goal) or a red screen (No-Goal). After 2 seconds and only when the stimuli color changed from red to purple, subjects were instructed to reach-and-touch the ball (Goal Movement) or to decide on their own where to touch (No-Goal Movement). 72 trials per condition were recorded. EEG signals were recorded using 60 passive electrodes and sampled at 512 Hz.

Independent component analysis (ICA) was performed for artefact removal: components representing eye movements and muscle activity were rejected. To extract relevant low-frequency time-domain features, data were down sampled to 16 Hz, common average referenced and band-pass filtered from 0.3 to 3 Hz with a zero-phase 4th order Butterworth filter. Classification was done using a random forest binary classifier; accuracies were calculated for each time-point and validated using 10x5-fold cross-validation. To score significantly above the chance level, 64.7% had to be reached (p=0.01, Bonferroni corrected for multiple tests over the trial length). Fig.

1 shows the time-course of the classification accuracies when discriminating Goal Movement and No-Goal Movement. Accuracies rise above the chance level after both first and second cues. After the GO cue (second cue), the average accuracy peaks immediately after movement onset with 67%. Here, 4 out of 10 subjects show accuracies above 80%, and all subjects are above the chance level. Also interestingly, 3 of the subjects show high accuracies even 2 seconds after movement onset.

Figure 1. Classification accuracies when discriminating Goal and No-Goal Movements, time-locked at movement onset (t=0s). The first 2 vertical lines correspond to the average time-points when the 1st and the 2nd cue appeared, in respect to movement onset. The thick black line corresponds to the grand-average accuracy.

Discussion: Our results show that there are differences between goal-directed and nongoal-directed movements when time-locking at movement onset. Namely, the motor-related cortical potentials – after the second cue- show different amplitudes between conditions. These differences are discriminable in a single-trial classification.

Future work will be to investigate whether similar results are obtained with neuroprostheses end-users and movement imagination (MI). If so, this information could be useful to establish activation thresholds, or even by instructing the subjects to imagine the kinesthetic MI associated with a target. We hypothesize that this instruction, combined with movement decoding at the kinematic level, could additionally improve classification accuracies.

Significance: The results contribute to the goal of our research: a naturally-controlled BCI neuroprostheses.

Furthermore, we encourage the BCI community to explore the neural correlates behind goal-directed movements and how recent neurophysiological findings in action planning (e.g. [2]) can be of practical interest for BCIs.

Acknowledgements: This work is supported by the EU ICT Programme Project H2020-643955, “MoreGrasp”

and the ERC Consolidator Grant “Feel Your Reach”.

References

[1] Pereira J, Ofner P, Muller-Putz G.R. Goal-directed or aimless? EEG differences during the preparation of a reach-and-touch task. In Proceedins of the 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE/EMBS, 1488-1491. IEEE, 2015.

[2] Aflalo T, Kellis S, Klaes C, Lee B, Shi Y, Pejsa K, Shanfield K, Hayes-Jackson S, Aisen Heck C and Liu C. Decoding motor imagery from the posterior parietal cortex of a tetraplegic human. Science, 348(6237), pp.906-910,2015.

DOI: 10.3217/978-3-85125-467-9-67 Proceedings of the 6th International Brain-Computer Interface Meeting, organized by the BCI Society

Published by Verlag der TU Graz, Graz University of Technology, sponsored by g.tec medical engineering GmbH 67

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

Many of the ideas on goal-directed behaviors as presented by lames, McDougall, Ach, Lewin. and the behaviorists, have been absorbed into modern goal theories, whereby

Figure 1: Our system provides visual model comparison by (1) showing side-by-side the classification landscapes of different models in linked panels after applying MDS projection on

We assessed participants' commitment to attain the original, lower or- der goal by asking them how disappointed they would feel it they failed to reach this goal. Participants

It is notable that we did not observe an auto-motive effect (Bargh, 1990) in the irrelevant implementation intention condition; that is, situational activation of the goal to

ical adherence is that people walk away from a health care provider (or from medical instructions obtained elsewhere) with a strong intention (goal) to act on the advice or

In the following, we explore how the cultural values of collectivism versus individualism, high versus low power differential, and strong versus weak uncertainty avoidance might

Research on goal setting observed that mentally contrasting a desired future outcome with obstacles of present reality leads to goal commitments to reach this outcome in

In conclusione, una funzione di autocorrelazione temporale ci dice per quanto tempo una certa proprietà del sistema permane prima che venga mediata a zero dalle fluttuazioni