• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Anzeige von Tense, Aspect and Negation (TAN) in Ìgáṣí

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Anzeige von Tense, Aspect and Negation (TAN) in Ìgáṣí"

Copied!
16
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Je ̣̀lílì Adéwálé Adéoyè (Adó-Èkìtì)

Abstract

Studies that are available on Ìgás̩í have only classified the speech form as one of the speech forms under the Ako̩ko̩id language cluster but none has examined tense, aspect and negation in the speech form. The present study provides a descriptive account of tense, aspect and negation in the speech form. It maintains that tense in Ìgáṣí polarises future and non-future. It establishes, among other things, that the speech form uses the pre-verbal particle á to mark its future tense and demonstrates that aspects in the speech form are divided into perfect and imperfect. The study also claims that Ìgáṣí has three basic negative morphemes which are kpa, sẹ and àge ̣̀ and shows that the future tense, perfect and habitual aspects have overt morphemes that reflect their presence in negative sentences. It is further claimed that àge ̣̀ which is divisible into nominal prefix (à) and negator (ge ̣̀) functions as the lexical negator in the speech form. Data in this study were obtained from native speakers of Ìgás̩í through oral interviews and their responses were recorded. It is hoped that this study, throws more light on the relationship among the functional categories (tense, aspect and negation) in Ìgás̩í speech form and document its syntax for pos- terity as nothing has been in that category.

1 Introduction

Functional categories are words without idiosyncratic descriptive content or properties; they primarily serve as information carriers coded in the grammatical properties of expression within the sentence Radford (1997). Functional categories belong to the closed class of words as a result, new entries either through lexical borrowing or morphological derivations are not al- lowed into their class Ilori (2010). Scholars such as Awoyale (1995) have identified the follow- ing as functional categories in languages: preposition, determiner, conjunction, complemen- tizer, tense, aspect, modal, agreement markers, negators, focus markers andgenitive markers.

In this present paper, we examine three functional categories in Ìgáṣí namely: tense, aspect and negation for the purpose of documentation.

Ìgáṣí is a speech form spoken in one of the communities in Àkókó North West LocalGovern- ment Area of Oǹdó State. The community is bounded by Erìtì, Àjọwá and Arigidi. Scholars such as Hoffman (1974) have classified the speech form as the Northern Akokoid Cluster while Akinkugbe (1976) refers to it as Akokoid. Capo (1989) in his work proposes Àm̀gbè as the nomenclature for the speech form and Fadoro (2010, 2012) regards the speech form as Arigidi O ̀wo ̀n. However, Olaogun (2016) proposes Njo-Koo language for a group of mutually intelli- gible speech forms formally known as Amgbe/Arigidi cluster spoken in six towns (Òkè-àgbè, Ìgáṣí, Àjọwá, Arigidi and Erúṣú) in the Northwest of Àkókó in Oǹdó State. The six towns are

(2)

made up of nine settlements which are Ìgáṣí, Arigidi, Erúṣú, Oyín and Urò in Àjọwá and Àfá, Ògè, Àjè, Udò in Òkè-àgbè. Very little is published on Ìgás̩í. The available works on the speech form are on classification without any thorough exploration into the structures of the speech form. This is not to say that there are no myriads of scholarly works on tense, aspect and nega- tion on Akokoidlects. However, no work exists or has been published on tense and aspect in Ìgáṣí.

This present paper is an effort to fill this gap and it is prompted by Crystal’s (2000) clarion call that linguists should explore endangered languages for the purpose of documentation before the languages go into extinction.

2 Tense

Tense relates events to the time of an action and it specifies the time of the event. It creates a link between the time of an action and the period of utterance. Lyons (1979:304) notes that the essential characteristic of the category of tense is that it relates the time of an action, event or state of affairs referred to in the sentence to the time of utterance, the time of utterance being now. Comrie (1985) claims that tense is a grammaticalised expression of location in time. Oma- mor (1982) identifies three points with reference to time; the points are retrospective point (RP), the time anterior to the time of initiation of speech, anticipated point (AP), the time posterior to the point of initiation of speech and the point present (PP), the point of initiation of speech. The summation from the definitions shows that tense relates the time of an action, event or state of affairs in languages. Having said this, the next section will dedicated to the examination of how tense relates the time of an action, event and states of affairs in Ìgáṣí.

2.1 Past tense in Ìgáṣí

Past tense signify event that occur prior to the time of the utterance. Yuka and Omorege (2011) state that past tense signals an event frame that proceeds the moment of speech. In Ìgáṣí, there is no overt phonetically visible morpheme for marking past tense; the past action is assumed to reflect on the verb in the sentence because it shows a completed action. See the examples below:

1a Adé vè

Adé go

‘Adé went.’

1b Òjó jwu ìtí

Òjó eat yam

‘Òjó ate yam.’

1c Bo ̣̀dé di bàtà

Bo ̀dé buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé bought shoes.’

1d Dúpe ̣́ ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ called me.’

It is evident in 1 that the action expressed by the verb is anterior to the time of the utterance.

The verbs in the sentences show a completed action prior to the time of discussion. A careful observation shows that no item is phonetically visible between the subject NP and the verb in 1 above. Interestingly, this situation is not strange to Benue Congo languages. It is reported in

(3)

Bamgbose (1990) that Yorùbá does not have overt marker for past tense. Consider the following examples:

2a Délé rí wọn Délé see 3rd PL OBJ

‘Délé saw them.’

2b A ra aṣọ

1st PL buy clothes

‘We bought clothes.’

Bamgboṣe (1990:167) Similarly, Ogunmodimu (2013) also reports that Àhàn language has no overt morpheme for marking past tense; see the examples below:

3a gbe usu NP plant yam

‘I planted yam.’

3b Kó lé ode Ko ̣́lé build house

‘Kolé built a house.’

Ogunmodimu (2013) From the examples 2 and 3 in Àhàn and Yorùbá, one notices that null phonetic item for past tense is not strange to the Defoid group of the Benue Congo languages. Moreover, to locate the appropriate time of the past time adverbs such as inúra‘yesterday’ òrun go ̣́rin’ ‘this morning’

o ̣̀se ̣́ ne ̣́ kọja ‘last week’ are often employed. It must be noted, however, that African languages such as Nweh morphologically distinguish between the past tense types, that is, immediate past (past within ‘today’), near past (past restricted to ‘yesterday’) and distant (remote) past (any time in the past prior to ‘yesterday’), Nkemnji (1995).

2.2 Future tense

The future tense locates events in some time ahead from the moment of speech (Yuka andOmo- rege 2011). Future tense is morphologically marked in Ìgáṣí, the morphemeáis used to mark future actions and it precedes the verb in the sentences. This is illustrated in the examples below:

4a Adé vè

Adé FUT go

‘Adé will go.’

4b Òjó ju ìtí

Òjó FUT eat yam

‘Òjó will eat yam.’

4c Bo ̣̀dé á di bàtà

Bo ̀dé FUT buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé will buy shoe.’

4d Dúpe ̣́ á ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ FUT call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ will call me.’

(4)

As shown in 4 the actions indicated by the verbs in the sentences are posterior to the time of the utterance. The presence of the preverbal particle (á) which precedes the verbs in the exam- ples 4 shows that the events discussed in the sentences indicate future occurrences. It can be observed that 1 and 4 are declarative sentences in Ìgáṣí. The occurrence of the preverbal particle in 7 and its non-occurrence in 4 may tentatively prompt an argument that declarative sentences can be divided into two based on their tenses (future or non-future). However, the division of tenses into future and non-future is common to some Defoidlanguages and their dialects. Ogun- modimu (2013) claims, that in Àhàn, affirmative sentences are divided into future and non- future tense. Similarly, Bamgbose (1990) notices a similar situation in Yorùbá. However, Nkemnji (1995) claims that future tense are graduated in Nweh where he identified three types of future tense type, that is, immediate (today) future, near (tomorrow) future and distant (re- mote) future.

3 Aspect

Aspect is one of the functional categories that is attested in Ìgásí. According to Huddleston and Pullum (2002:117) aspect applies to a system where the basic meaning has to do with the inter- nal temporal constituency of the situation. Aspects in Ìgáṣí are divided basically into two namely: imperfect and perfect.

3.1 Progressive aspect in Ìgáṣí

Progressive aspect primarily shows that the action specified by the verb is ongoing as at the time of the utterance or was on-going in the past. Obiamalu (2015) succinctly describes pro- gressive aspect as an on-going process at the time of speaking, traditionally referred to as pre- sent continuous. It could also refer to an on-going action at a point in time in the past, tradition- ally referred to as past continuous. Ìgáṣí uses ró as a progressive marker. The preverbal particle occurs between the subject DP and the verb as exemplified in the examples below:

5a Adé ró vè

Adé PROG go

‘Adé is going.’

5b Òjó ró jwu ìtí

Ojó PROG eat yam

‘Òjó is eating yam.’

5c Bo ̣̀dé ró di bàtà

Bo ̀dé PROG buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé is buying shoes.’

5d Dúpe ̣́ ró ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ PROG call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ is calling me.’

As shown in 5 the presence of the progressive aspectual marker in the examples indicated that the action of the verb is happening as at the time of the utterance, that is, the action is on-going when the utterance is made. Progressive aspect sometimes may have the semantics of habitual aspect based on the context of use. As stated earlier, progressive aspect can indicate past ac- tions/events. See the examples below:

(5)

6a' Olú ró ǹṣẹ úvọ ìfọ ni vadi Olú PROG sleep hand duration CPA arrive

‘Olú was sleeping when I arrived.’

6a'' Ìfọ ni vadi olú ró ǹṣẹ ùvọ

Duration PART arrive Olú PROG sleep hand

‘When I arrived, Olú was sleeping.’

6b' Adé ró fo achọ inúra ìfo ni oji

Adé PROG wash clothes yesterday duration CPA rain ró tèé

PROG fall

‘Adé was washing clothes yesterday when rain was falling.’

6b'' Ìfọ ni oji ró tèé Adé ró

duration PART rain PROG fall Adé PROG

achọ inúra clothes yesterday

‘when rain was falling, Adé was washing clothes yesterday’

As illustrated in 6 the progressive aspect is not limited to on-going actions in the present but also in the past actions. The examples in 5 and 6, represent two events while one is foreground- ing the other is backgrounding. In effect, one can argue that progressive aspect in Ìgáṣí relate on-going actions or events in the present in 5 while 6 shows an on-going event in the past, using the same ró as progressive marker. The examples in 6 present two events, while 6b' is back- grounding, 6a''is foregrounding. The same thing is applicable to 6b' and 6b''. The particle ni that occurs immediately after ìfọ in the sentences is complimentizer adjunct.

3.2 Habitual Aspect

Habitual aspect indicates an action that occurs often or regularly. Fabunmi (2009) refers to habitual aspect as an action with an indefinite occurrence. ré is used to mark this particular action in Ìgáṣí. Consider the examples below:

7a Adé ré vè aja Adé HAB go market

‘Adé used to go to the market.’

7b Òjó ré ju ìti

Òjó HAB eat yam

‘Òjó used to eat yam.’

7c Bo ̣̀dé ré di bàtà

Bo ̀dé HAB buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé used to buy shoes.’

7d Dúpe ̣́ ré ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ HAB call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ used to call me.’

In 7, the actions of the verb described by the habitual marker are events that occur often. It is important to note that Ìgáṣí has a peculiar habitual aspect marker which is monosyllabic unlike some Defoid languages such Standard Yorùbá and Yorùbá Àkókó where the habitual aspect polysyllabic, that is, máa ń and mà í respectively (see Fabunmi 2009).

(6)

3.3 Perfect aspect

Perfect aspect indicates a completed action of the verb. In Ìgáṣí, the preverbal particle kais used as the perfect aspect. See the examples below:

8a Adé ka vè

Adé PERF go

‘Adé has gone.’

8b Òjó ka jwu ìtí

Òjó PERF eat yam

‘Òjó has eating yam.’

8c Bo ̣̀dé ka di bàtà

Bo ̀dé PERF buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé has bought shoes.’

8d Dúpe ̣́ ka ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ PERF call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ has called me.’

As shown in 8, the presence of the preverbal particle indicates that the action of the verb is completed and it is in the past because the action of the verb is anterior to the time of theutter- ance. It is important to note that present perfect tense is not attested in Ìgáṣí just like Yorùbá

where there is no visible morpheme for present tense.

3.4 The relationship between tense and aspect in Ìgáṣí

The co-occurrence of pre-verbal particles such as tense and aspect among others has been re- ported in Benue-Congo languages such as Yorùbá, Ìgbò and Àhàn. Ìgáṣí, the language, under discussion is not excluded as aspects and tense are allowed to co-occur in well-formed sen- tences. Based on this fact, the examination of the relationship between aspects and tense and their order of occurrence in sentences becomes necessary in this study.

3.4.1 Co-occurrence of aspects in Ìgáṣí

Two aspectual markers are allowed to co-occur in a well-formed sentence. Perfect aspect can co-occur freely with both progressive and habitual aspects. Consider the examples below:

9a Adé ka ró ko ǹsẹ

Adé PERF PROG sing song

‘Adé has been singing song (s).’

9b Olú ka ró ǹsẹ

Olú PERF PROG sleep

‘Olú has been sleeping.’

10a Adé ka ré si de ̣̀nde ̣̀

Adé PERF HAB run surpass

‘Adé used to run often.’

10b Adé ka ré jà de ̣̀nde ̣̀

Adé PERF HAB fight surpass

‘Adé used to fight often.’

11a *Adé ró ka ko ǹsẹ

Adé PROG PERF sing song

(7)

11b *Adé ré ka vè

Adé HAB PERF go 11c *Olú ró ré vè

Olú PROG HAB go 11d *Olú ré ró vè

Olú HAB PROG go

As shown in 9 and 10, perfect aspect occurs freely with progressive and habitual aspect in the sentences. It is observed in the data that there is order of precedence in terms of the occurrence of the aspectual markers in the sentences. Perfect aspect occurs first before any other aspectual marker; however, the reversal of this ordering may lead to ungrammaticality as shown in 11a and 11b. Note that the co-occurrence of Ìgáṣí aspectual marker is restricted, apart from the perfect aspect that occurs freely, no other aspectual marker can co-occur in a well formed sen- tence; hence the ungrammaticality of 11c and 11d. The occurrence of two aspectual markers in a well-formed sentence is not strange in Defoid languages. Bamgbose (1990) reports that two aspectual markers are allowed to co-occur in Yorùbá sentence as illustrated in the examples below:

12a Wo ̣́n ti lọ

3PL PERF PROG go

‘They have been going.’

12b Wo ̣́n ti máa ń lọ

3PL PERF HAB go

‘They are used to go.’

3.4.2 Co-occurrences of Tense and Aspect in Ìgáṣí

Tense and aspectual markers are allowed to co-occur in a well-formed sentence in Ìgáṣí. It must be noted that the arrangement of the preverbal particles are not haphazardly done, they are arranged one after the other in a pattern that is considered permissible on account of style and meaning in the language. Consider the examples below:

13a Olú ka á de Èkò

Olú PERF FUT get Lagos

‘Olú would have got to Lagos.’

13b Akin ka á jwíhọ

Akin PERF FUT eat food

‘Akin would have eaten food.’

13c *Bayo á ka jwíhọ

Bayo FUT PERF eat food

As evident in 13, it can be observed that the perfect aspect precedes the future tense marker in the sentence. However, the reversal of the arrangement as shown in 13c leads to ill-formed sentence. Observe also that it is only perfect aspect marker that can co-occur with tense marker in a well-formed sentence. The occurrence of other aspectual markers with tense in Ìgáṣí will result in ungrammatical sentence. From the arrangement of the future tense and perfect aspect in examples 13a and 13b one can say that tense establishes the time framework and aspect sets out how the situation is distributed within the time framework. Having explained tense and

(8)

aspect in relations to verbs in Ìgáṣí, the next section will be dedicated to the relationship be- tween negation, tense, aspect and focus in Ìgáṣí.

4 Negation

Negation implies the contradiction of the assertions made in a sentence. Crystal (2008) asserts that negation is a process or construction in a grammatical or semantic analysis which typically expresses the contradiction of some or all of a sentence meaning. Scholars have divided nega- tion into two; they are constituent negation and sentence negation. Constituent negation pre- supposes the contradiction of some or parts of a sentence while sentence negation implies the negation of a whole sentence (see Payne 1992). Ìgáṣí manifests both the constituent and sen- tence negation. Ìgáṣí has the three basic negative markers. They are kpa (‘not’), sẹ (‘don’t’) and ge ̣̀ (‘un-‘). Note that perfective, progressive, habitual and future actions have overt morphemes that show their presence in negative sentence. Consider the examples below:

4.1 Sentence negation (indicating past action)

Sentence negation which indicates past action implies denying the action of the verb that is anterior to the time of the utterance. This is shown in the sentences below:

14a Adé kpa vè

Ade NEG1 go

‘Ade didn’t go.’

14b Òjó kpa ju ìtí

Òjó NEG1 eat yam

‘Òjó didn’t eat yam.’

14c Bo ̣̀dé kpa di bàtà

Bo ̀dé NEG 1 buy shoes

‘Bo ̀dé didn’t buy shoes.’

14d Dúpe ̣́ kpa swè mí

Dúpe ̣́ NEG1 call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ didn’t call me.’

In 14, it is observed that the negative marker kpa surfaces between the subject DP and the verb.

The negative marker denies/negates the assertion of the verb in the sentence. It is important to note that the tense of the sentence is reflected on the negative marker. This observation is similar to Bamgbose’s (1967) claim with respect to Yorùbá. He submits that in Yorùbá sentences where a negator is followed by a verb in the positive unmarked tense; it normally indicates the un- marked tense. That is, the negativemarker in the sentence reflects the tense which could be past or non-past irrespective of whether the verb is an “action” verb or not.

4.1.1 Sentence negation (indicating future action)

Sentence negation which shows future action presupposes that the action of the verb that is posterior to the time of the utterance is negated. This is illustrated below:

15a Adé kpà romi vè

Adé NEG 1 FUT go

‘Adé will not go.’

(9)

15b Òjó kpà romi ju ìtí

Òjó NEG 1 FUT eat yam

‘Òjó will not eat yam.’

15c Bo ̣̀dé kpà romi di bàtà

Bo ̀dé NEG1 FUT buy shoes

‘Bo ̀dé will not buy shoes.’

15d Dúpe ̣́ kpà romi ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ NEG1 FUT call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ will not call me.’

In 15, there is a phonetically visible element (romi) that occurs after the negative marker be- tween the subject DP and the verb which shows that the negative sentence is discussing a future action. It is observed that the form of future marker has a different shape in the negative sen- tence as shown in examples 15. In the positive sentence in 4 the future marker is á and it pre- cedes the verb but in the negative sentence in 15 the shape of the future tense marker is romi.

The difference in the shape of future marker as shown in 4 and 15, is not strange in languages.

A similar situation is reported in Yorùbá. Consider the Yorùbá examples below:

16a Olú máa lọ

Olú FUT go

‘Olú will go.’

16b Olú kò nìí lọ

Olú NEG FUT go

‘Olú will not go.’

In the Yorùbá examples, the shape of the future maker is deferent in the negative sentence. It is máain the positive sentence as shown in 16a while the morpheme is nìíin the negative sentence in 16b. A keen observer will also notice that Ìgás̩í negative marker carries (kpà) alow tone in 15 compared to its basic mid tone as shown in 14. The simple explanation for the change in the tone as noted in 15, can probably be attributed to the reflection of the future tense in the sen- tences which has resulted in tone lowering.

4.1.2 Sentence Negation (indicating progressive aspect)

Sentence negation which shows progressive aspect presupposes that the action of the verb that is on-going as at the time of the utterance or in the past is negated. See examples 17 below:

17a Adé kpà vè

Adé NEG1 go

‘Adé is not going.’

17b Òjó kpà jwu ìtí

Òjó NEG1 eat yam

‘Òjó is not eating yam.’

17c Bo ̣̀dé kpà di bàtà

Bo ̀dé NEG1 buy shoes

‘Bo ̀dé is not buying shoes.’

(10)

17d Dúpe ̣́ kpà ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ NEG1 call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ didn’t call me.’

It is evident in 17 that the action of the verb is negated. It is also observed that there is no overt manifestation of progressive marker in the sentences as shown in the positive counterparts in 5.

As rightly observed in Bamgbose (1990), when Yorùbá verb without an overt marker of tense is negated in a sentence, the tense is always reflected on the negative marker. This process is applicable to aspect without overt marker in Ìgáṣí as shown in 17, the aspect is reflected on the negative marker. A careful observation shows that the negative marker in the sentences that contain progressive aspect in 17 and the past tense in 14 is the same. The reason for this is not farfetched, Ìgáṣí has no visible morpheme for past action and the progressive aspect with visible morpheme in the positive sentence may on several occasions refer to present events because it indicates an ongoing action at the time of theutterance (the on-going events can either be in the past or present). However, in 17, the marker of progressive aspect is not shown in the negative sentences. This, we believe, must have prompted the same negative marker for the two sen- tences but the negative marker indicates two different things in the two sentences while one indicates past action, the other shows an on-going action. This argument may lead us to suggest that kpa in the two sentences are homonyms or that same element (kpa) performs the two func- tions. This observation is not peculiar to Ìgáṣí alone, a similar situation is observed in Yorùbá.

In Yorùbá, sentences which reflect past tense and progressive aspect have the same negative marker. See the examples below in Yorùbá:

18a Adé lọ Adé went. positive sentence (past) 18b Adé kò lọ Adé did not go. negative sentence (past) 18c Adé ń lọ Adé is going. positive progressive 18d Adé ko lọ Adé is not going. negative progressive 4.1.3 Sentence Negation (indicating perfect aspect)

Sentence negation which has a reflection of perfective aspect means that the action of the verb that is temporarily completed is negated. This is illustrated in 25 below:

19a Adé kpà ma vè

Adé NEG1 PERF go

‘Adé has not gone.’

19b Òjó kpà ma jwu ìtí

Òjó NEG1 PERF eat yam

‘Òjó has not eaten yam.’

19c Bo ̣̀dé kpà ma di bàtà

Bo ̀dé NEG1 PERF buy shoes

‘Bo ̀dé has not bought shoes.’

19d Dúpe ̣́ kpà ma ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ NEG1 PERF call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ has not call me.’

In 19, the sentences indicate that the perfective actions of the verbs are negated. One observes that the form of the perfect aspect in the negative sentence is macompared to its positive coun- terparts in 8 wherekais used as perfect aspect. It is also noticed that the tone of the negative

(11)

marker changes from mid to high. The simple explanation for this tonal change can probably be that Ìgáṣí prohibits the contiguous occurrence of two mid tone preverbal particles. This may probably be the rationale for raising the mid tone of the negative marker to high tone when it is followed by another preverbal particle with a mid-tone.

4.1.4 Sentence Negation (indicating habitual aspect)

Sentence negation which reflects habitual aspect simply suggests that the action of the verb that indicates indefinite action is negated. This is exemplified in (26) below:

20a Adé kpà ré vè aja Adé NEG1 HAB go market

‘Adé did not use to go market.’

20b Òjó kpà ré jwu ìtí

Òjó NEG1 HAB eat yam

‘Òjó did not use to eat yam.’

20c Bo ̣̀dé kpà ré di bàtà

Bo ̀dé NEG1 HAB buy shoe

‘Bo ̀dé did not use to buy shoes.’

20d Dúpe ̣́ kpà ré ṣwè mí

Dúpe ̣́ NEG1 HAB call 1st SG OBJ

‘Dúpe ̣́ did not use to call me.’

As shown in 20 above, it is observed that the predicates of the sentences which reflect habitual aspects are negated. It is evident in the examples, that the shape of the habitual marker remains constant and its high tone is retained just like the positive counterparts. Worthy of explanation is the tonal variations observed in the negative marker indicating future action and perfective aspect that is not applicable to the negative marker in 20. The simple reason for this is that the habitual marker maintains its high tone just like as it were in the positive sentence. Thus, the consistency of the tone of the negative and the habitualmarkers block the process of tone low- ering and raising of the negative marker as illustrated in 15 and 19.

4.2 Focus Negation

Focus negation implies negating the emphasized constituent in a sentence. The negated constit- uent could either be the subject, object, verb, adverb, or adjective in a sentence (Adeoye 2018).

In Ìgáṣí, the focused constituent negated is always preceded by the negative morpheme kpá

(neg) si (negative particle). It must be noted, that Ìgáṣí has two focus markers which occur in complementary distribution, while úwọn focuses only subject NP, win focuses any other con- stituents in the sentence. Consider the examples below:

4.2.1 Subject NP negation

Subject NP focus negation, entails the movement of the subject NP to sentence initial position and it is preceded by negative marker kpá (NEG) si (negative particle) and followed by the focus marker. See the examples below:

(12)

21a Kpá si Adé úwọn vè aja NEG 1 PART Adé FOC go market

‘It wasn’t Ade that went to market.’

21b Kpá si Òjó úwọn jwu ìtí

NEG1 PART Òjó FOC eat yam

‘It wasn’t Òjó that ate yam.’

21c Kpá Si Bo ̣̀dé úwọn di bàtà

NEG1 PART Bo ̀dé FOC buy shoes

‘It wasn’t Bode that bought the shoes.’

21d Kpá si Dúpe ̣́ úwọn ṣwè mí

NEG 1 PART Dúpe ̣́ FOC call 1st SG OBJ

‘It wasn’t Dúpe ̣́ that called me.?’

4.2.2 Object NP Negation

Object NP focus negation presupposes that the object of the verb is moved within the IP to sentence initial position and it is preceded by the focus negative marker and followed by the focus marker. This is illustrated below:

22a Kpá si aja win Adé vè

NEG1 PART market FOC Adé go

‘It wasn’t the market that Adé that went to.’

22b Kpá si ìtí win Òjó jwu NEG1 PART yam FOC Òjó eat

‘It wasn’t yam that Òjó ate.’

22c Kpá si bàtà win Bo ̣̀dé dà

NEG 1 PART shoes FOC Bo ̀dé buy

‘It wasn’t shoes that Bo ̀dé bought.’

22d Kpá si emí win Dúpe ̣́ swè

NEG1 PART I FOC Dúpe ̣́ call

‘It wasn’t me that Dúpe ̣́ called.’

4.2.3 Verb Negation

Verb focus negation involves the process of copying the verb and the addition of a prefix to it before for purpose of nominalisation. The nominalised verb is moved to sentence initial position where it is preceded by the negative marker. See the examples below:

23a Kpá si àve win Adé vè

NEG 1 PART going FOC Adé go

‘It wasn’t the act of going that Adé performed.’

23b Kpá Si àju win Òjó jwu ìtí

NEG 1 PART eating FOC Òjó eat yam

‘It wasn’t eating that Òjó ate yam.’

23c Kpá si àda win Bo ̣̀dé di bàtà

NEG 1 PART buying FOC Bo ̀dé buy shoe

‘It wasn’t buying that Bo ̀dé bought shoes.’

(13)

23d Kpá si àṣwe win Dúpe ̣́ ṣwè mí

NEG 1 PART calling FOC Dúpe ̣́ call 1st SG OBJ

‘It wasn’t calling that Dúpe ̣́ called me.’

As shown in 21, 22 and 23 different constituents are focused and negated. In 21, the subject NP is focused and moved to sentence initial position and it is preceded by the negative marker kpá

(neg) si (negative particle). In 22, the focused object NP of the verb is negated while the verb is left stranded. In 23, the focused verb is negated; the verb is copied and it takes a nominal prefix (à) before it is moved to sentence initial position and it is also preceded by the negative marker. A careful observer will notice that the choice of the focus markers in the IP is dependent on the position occupied by constituents.

4. 3 Lexical Negation

Lexical negation in Ìgáṣí involves the nominal prefixàand lexical negator ge ̣̀ to the verb. The prefix changes the lexical category of the verb to a noun. Consider the examples below:

24a ge ̣̀ vè Àge ̣̀vè

Nominal Prefix NEG2 go The act of not going

24b ge ̣̀ ǹṣẹ Àge ̣̀ǹṣẹ

Nominal Prefix NEG2 sleep The act of not sleeping

24c ge ̣̀ gwo ̣́ Àge ̣̀gwo ̣́

Nominal Prefix NEG2 drink The act of not drinking

24d ge ̣̀ vadi Àge ̣̀vadi

Nominal Prefix NEG2 return The act of not returning

24e Ge ̣̀ ǹgba Àge ̣̀ǹgba

Nominal Prefix NEG2 wise The act of not wise.

In 24, it is observed that the prefix is a disyllabic item and the occurrence of the element is restricted to verb negation in isolation. It is assumed in this study that à is the nominal prefix while ge ̣̀is the negator. This assumption is borne out of that fact that nominalisation process in Ìgás̩í involves à prefixation to a verb. Thus, in the derivation of the examples in 24, it is ob- served thatgẹ̀ is the prefixed to the verb to yield Negative Phrase. Afterwards, the nominal prefix à is the added to Verb Phrase to give Nominal Phrase as the output. This implies that the prefix à is the head. It changes the lexical category from a Verb to a Noun.

4.4 Imperative Negation

Imperative sentence indicates command or order and the subject of the sentence is always the second person singular or R-expression. Ìgáṣí uses sẹ that precedes verbs for this purpose. See the examples below:

25a Sẹ vè

NEG3 go Don’t go.

(14)

25b Sẹ dédí

NEG3 steal Don’t steal.

25c Sẹ ǹsẹ

NEG3 sleep Don’t sleep.

25d Sẹ jwíhọ

NEG3 eat Don’t eat.

25e Sẹ gwoùji NEG3 drink water Don’t drink water.

As shown in 25, the imperative negator always precedes verb just like the lexical negator. One needs to explain that while lexical negator is a prefix and it changes the lexical category of the verb that gets attached to it, a noun. The imperative negator on the other hand, does not change the status of verb to a noun but maintains its status as a sentence despite the ellipsis of the subject NP. It must be noted that imperative negator is restrictive in its occurrence and it is in complementary distribution with kpa. It is also observed that se occurs predominantly in an imperative sentence (command), while kpa with other designated morphemes occurs elsewhere.

5 Conclusion

In this article, we have examined the structure of tense, aspect and negation in Ìgáṣí. It is shown in the paper that tense in Ìgáṣí polarises future and non-future tense. It is further established, that the speech form uses the preverbal particle á to mark its future tense. It is also demonstrated that the speech form has designated morphemes for future tense, perfective, progressive and habitual aspects in negative sentences just like their positive counterparts. The study claims that Ìgáṣí has three basic negative markers which are kpa, sẹ and àge ̣̀. It is shown in this study that two preverbal particles with mid tone cannot occur side by side, as a result, the negative marker undergoes tone lowering or tone raisin

List of Abbreviations

1st, 2nd, 3rd first, second, and third person pronoun respectively CPA complementizer adjunct

FOC focus marker

FUT future tense for affirmative (á) and negative future tense (romi) IP inflectional phrase

HAB habitual aspect marker for both affirmative and negative NEG1 sentence negation

NEG2 lexical negation NEG3 imperative negation

NP noun phrase

(15)

OBJ object

PART particle for negative particle (si)

PERF perfective aspect marker for affirmative (ka) resp. negative sentences (ma)

PL plural

PROG progressive aspect marker

SG singular

VP verb phrase

References

Adeoye, Jelili (2018): “The Structure of Negation in Uwu” Kansas Working Papers in Linguis- tics 39: 40–56.

Ajiboye, Jacob (2012): “À-ì-Derived Nominals in Yorùbá.” Lagos Notes and Records 18/1:

117–138.

Akínkúgbé, Olufemi (1976): “An Internal Classification of the Yoruboid Group (Yoruba, Ise- kiri, Igala)”. Journal of West African Languages 11: 1–19.

Awóbùlúyì, Oladele (1992): “Aspect of Contemporary Standard Yorùbá in Dialectological Per- spective” in Akínwùmi, Ishola (ed.) New Findings in Yorùbá Studies. J. F. Ọdúnjọ Memorial Lectures Organizing Committee: 1–76.

Awóbùlúyì, Oladel (1998): “Àwọn Ẹ̀ka-èdè Yorùbá”. Paper read at Yorùbá Studies Association of Nigeria, Pastoral Institute, Bodija, Ibadan.

Awobuluyi, Oladele (2005): “Mofiimukanṣoṣoni ‘àì’ àbí meji”. Journal of Yoruba Studies As- sociation of Nigeria. 3/1: 1–6.

Awobuluyi, Oladele (2008): Ẹkọ Iṣẹda-Ọrọ. Akure (Nigeria): Montern Paperbacks.

Awoyale, Yiwola (1995) “The Role of Functional Categories in Syntax: The Yoruba Case”, Owolabi Kola (ed.) Language in Nigeria: Essay in Honour of Ayo Bamgbose. Ibadan, Group Publishers: 113–127.

Bamgbose, Ayo (1967): A short Yoruba Grammar. Ibadan: HEB Bamgbose, Ayo (1990):

Fonoloji ati Girama Yoruba. Ibadan: UPL.

Capo, Hounkpati (1989): “Defoid”. In: Bendor-Samuel (ed.): The Niger-Congo Languages:

Classification and Description of Africa’s largest Language family. Lanham (Maryland), University Press of America: 2275–2900.

Constantine, Yuka/Esohe¸ Omoregbe (2011) “Tense and Aspect in Edo.” Journal of Linguistics Association of Nigeria 14/2: 365–377.

Comrie, Bernard (1985): Tense. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chomsky, Noam (1991): “Some Notes on Economy of Derivation and Representation”. In:

Robert, Freidin (ed.): Principles and Parameters in Comparative Grammar. Cambridge (MA), MIT Press: 417–454.

Chomsky, Noam (1993): “A Minimalist Program for Linguistic Theory”. In: Hale, Ken- neth/Keyser, Samuel Jay (eds.): View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge (MA) /London, MIT Press: 1–52.

Crystal, David (2000): Language Death. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

(16)

Crystal, David (2008): A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. 6th eds. Malden (MA) /Ox- ford: Blackwell.

Fabunmi, Felix (2009): A GSPG Structure of Aspect in Yorùbá Àkókó. Nordic Journal ofAfri- can Studies 18/4: 258–285.

Fadoro, Jacob (2010): Phonological and Lexical Variation in Akokoid. Unpublished PhD The- sis, Department of Linguistics and African Languages, University of Ibadan, Oyo State, Ni- geria-

Fadoro, Jacob (2012): “Towards Akokoid Orthographies” Papers in English and Linguistics (PEL), 13/2: 223–240.

Huddleston, Rodney/Pullum, Geoffrey (2002: 117): The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge University Press.

Hoffman, Carl (1974): The Language of Nigeria by Language Families. Mimeograph. Ibadan:

University of Ibadan.

Ìlọ̀rí, John (2010): Nominal Constructions in Igala and Yorùbá. PhD thesis, Adekunle Ajasin University, Akungba Akoko.

Obiamalu, Greg (2015): Functional Categories in Igbo: A minimalist perspective. Port Har- court: M & J Grand Orbit Communications.

Ogunmodimu, Morakinyo (2013): Tense, Aspect and Negation in Ahan. Hawaii University In- ternational Conference, Arts, Humanities and Social science, Honolulu. www. huichawaii.

org/assets/ogunmodimu_morakinyo_ahs_2013. pdf [22. 11. 2019].

Olaogun, Simeon (2016): Information Structural Categories of the Ǹjo ̣̀-Kóo Language in Akoko North-West of Ondo State, Nigeria. Unpublished PhD Thesis, University of Ibadan, Oyo State, Nigeria.

Olúmúyìwá, Temitope (2013): “Tense/Aspect and Negation in Mọ̀bà (A Yorùbá Dialect)”.

Research on Humanities and Social Sciences. 3/1:118–123.

Omamor, Augusta (1982): “Tense and Aspect in Isekiri”. JWAL XII 2: 95–129.

Payne, John (1992): “Negation”. In: Frawley, William (ed.): International Encyclopaedia of Lin- guistics. 3. Oxford, Oxford University Press: 75–76.

Nkemnji, Michael (1995): Heavy Pied Piping in Nweh. Unpublished PhD Thesis, University of California, Los Angeles.

Radford, Andrew (1997): Syntactic Theory and the Structure of English. Cambridge: Cam- bridge University Press.

Taiwo, Oye (2003): “Tense and Aspect in Ào”. In: Ndimele, Ozo-mekuri (ed.): Four decades in the study of languages and linguistics in Nigeria. A Festschrift for Kay Williamson. Port Harcourt, M & J Grand Orbit Communications: 773–790.

Uwalaka, Mary (1997): Ìgbò Grammar. Ibadan: The Pen Services.

Zanuttini, Raffaella (1996): “On the relevance of tense for sentential negation”. In: Belletti, Adriana/Rizzi, Luigi (eds.): Parameters and functional heads: Essays in comparative syn- tax. Oxford, Oxford University Press:181–207.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

”"“" ° enormous waste takes place, although if a chargeis madein accordance wit‘1 the quantity delivered it is said to provide an excuse for pe130ns to limit themselvesin

Deputy Prime Minister Bülent Arınç said on 4 April that the strong support for the Justice and Development Party (AKP) in the March 30 local elections is a crystal clear sign

the theoretical data for Au only polycrystalline sam- ples were taken into account: the monocrystals of Au seem to make s av very much outside the interval (43) and this can

 Most of the PAs in the Highland, for Example the Arsi Highland  Park  forms  the  water  shed  that  sustain  the  livelihood  of  millions  of  people  in 

Well, according to Hardin, safeguarding the global commons requires a top-down approach administered by prominent international organizations.. The line of reasoning here is

I Über den Zeitpunkt des Empfangs kann keine Aussage getroffen werden (Unbounded indeterminancy). I Über die Reihenfolge der Empfangenen Nachrichten wird im Aktorenmodell keine

Some time ago [1], we described measurements of the enthalpies of mixing for the liquid system water -f- acetic acid, carried out with a newly developed calorimeter based on

Following a discussion of a number of theoretical issues concerned with the relationship between aesthetics and cognition, the application of a Gibsonian view of perception to