• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Gap detection in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris).

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Gap detection in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). "

Copied!
8
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

J C omp Physiol A ( 1991) 168 : 469-476

Joumal of

C O ~ l [ ~ M " a t t v e and .-,-=,

Physiology A ~ '

9 Springer-Verlag 1991

Gap detection in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris).

III. Processing in the peripheral auditory system

Georg M. Klump* and Otto Gleich

Institut ffir Zoologie, Technische Universit/it Mfinchen, Lichtenbergstr. 4, W-8046 Garching, Federal Republic of Germany

Summary. Gap-detection thresholds were determined for single units in the cochlear ganglion and in auditory nerve fibres of the starling from responses to two broad- band noise bursts separated by a temporal gap of be- tween 0.4 and 204.8 ms. All 35 units showed a threshold within the range of gap sizes tested. The median mini- mum-detectable gap was 12.8 ms with the minimum be- ing 1.6 ms. A multiple regression analysis revealed that the size of the minimum-detectable gap was not signifi- cantly correlated with the neuron's CF, with its sharp- ness of tuning as given by its bandwidth 10 dB above threshold, or with its

QIOdB

value. Only the level of stim- ulation above the neuron's threshold showed a signifi- cant negative correlation with the size of the minimum- detectable gap. These results are discussed with respect to theoretical considerations of limits posed on temporal resolution by the characteristics of peripheral filters.

These findings are also discussed in the context of the coding of gaps at different levels of the starling's audito- ry system and in relation to psychoacoustic results in the starling on gap detection and time resolution de- scribed by temporal modulation transfer functions.

Key words: Bird - Hearing - Auditory nerve - Temporal resolution - Gap detection

Introduction

Temporal resolution in auditory systems has been stud- ied mainly with two experimental paradigms: the detec- tion of sinusoidal amplitude modulations and the detec- tion of single temporal gaps in broadband noise. Green and Forrest (1988) modified a model previously de- scribed by Viemeister (1979) to fit human psychoacous- Abbreviations: CF characteristic frequency; TW time window;

Qlod, the unit's characteristic frequency divided by the bandwidth 10 dB above threshold

* To whom offprint requests should be sent

tic data for the detection of temporal gaps and sinusoidal amplitude modulations. This model is composed of a bandpass filter followed by a half-wave rectifier and a lowpass filter. The output of the lowpass filter is moni- tored by a detection mechanism comparing minimum and maximum amplitude values in a specific time win- dow. Although their model fits the human psychoacous- tic data extremely well, it is not based directly on physio- logical evidence on frequency and time resolution in the auditory periphery. Animal models in which both the psychoacoustics and the neuronal coding of temporal patterns in broadband noise is studied may provide us with direct evidence on the relationship between neuro- nal coding in the auditory periphery implicit in the mod- els of temporal perception and the psychoacoustics of temporal resolution.

The European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) is one of the few experimental animals in which time resolution phenomena relevant for gap detection have been studied both psychoacoustically (Klump and Maier 1989) and neurophysiologically (Buchfellner et al. 1989) using ex- perimental paradigms comparable to those used in the study of temporal pattern perception in humans. This study provides information concerning the peripheral coding of temporal gaps in broadband noise and dis- cusses the results with respect to central coding of the same stimulus in the starling's auditory forebrain areas and to the psychoacoustics of gap detection.

The relation between gap thresholds measured psy- choacoustically and the peripheral coding of temporal gaps in broadband noise has been studied so far in 3 other animal species: an acridid grasshopper (Chort- hippus biguttulus, von Helversen 1972; Ronacher and Rrmer 1985), the goldfish (Carassius auratus, Fay 1985), and the Chinchilla. (Chinchilla laniger, e.g. Giraudi et al.

1980; Zhang et al. 1990). The present experiments ex- tend these observations to birds. An interspecific com- parison should reveal to what extent common mecha- nisms may be operating in the perception of rapid fluctu- ations of the signal envelope.

Psychoacoustic studies on gap detection in humans

(2)

470 G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection ( S h a i l e r a n d M o o r e 1983), c h i n c h i l l a (Chinchilla laniger,

S a l v i a n d A r e h o l e 1985), a n d in b u d g e r i g a r s a n d z e b r a f i n c h e s (Melopsittacus undulatus, Poephila guttata, O k - a n o y a a n d D o o l i n g 1990) h a v e s h o w n t h a t t i m e r e s o l u - t i o n v a r i e s w i t h t h e s p e c t r a l r a n g e o f t h e s i g n a l c o n t a i n - i n g t h e g a p . W i t h t h e e x c e p t i o n o f t h e r e s u l t s f r o m b u d - g e r i g a r s ( O k a n o y a a n d D o o l i n g 1990), t h e r e is a n i n v e r s e r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n t h e size o f t h e m i n i m u m d e t e c t a b l e g a p a n d t h e a n i m a l ' s c r i t i c a l b a n d w i d t h in t h e s p e c t r a l r a n g e o f t h e s i g n a l c o n t a i n i n g t h e t e m p o r a l i n f o r m a t i o n . T h i s s u g g e s t s t h a t t h e d u r a t i o n o f t h e t e m p o r a l r e s p o n s e o f t h e p e r i p h e r a l filters is r e s p o n s i b l e f o r t h e f r e q u e n c y - d e p e n d e n c e o f t e m p o r a l r e s o l u t i o n . W e s t u d i e d t h i s q u e s t i o n in t h e s t a r l i n g ' s p e r i p h e r a l n e u r o n s b y c o m p a r - i n g t h e n e u r o n ' s t u n i n g c h a r a c t e r i s t i c w i t h t h e size o f its m i n i m u m d e t e c t a b l e g a p .

Materials and methods

Preparation and recording procedure. Experiments described here were performed on 9 wild caught European starlings (Sturnus vul- garis), weighing between 70 and 90 g. The preparation for record- ing from primary auditory afferents in the starling has been de- scribed in detail (Manley et al. 1985) and is briefly summarized as follows. The birds were anaesthetized with an initial dose of Pentobarbital-Na (90 mg/kg body weight). Deep anaesthesia was maintained during the experiments by supplementary doses when a heart rate increase or when skeleton-muscle activity was observed in the electrocardiogram. The birds were artificially respirated and the core temperature kept at 4 0 _ 1 ~ with a regulated heating- pad.

We used two approaches to record primary auditory afferents.

In a first series of experiments, neurons (n=10) were recorded from the cochlear ganglion which was accessed via a dorsolateral approach through an opening in the recessus scalae tympani. In a second series of experiments, recordings were obtained from the primary auditory fibres of the VIIIth nerve (n = 25) without open- ing the cochlea. The course of primary auditory-nerve fibres be- tween the foramen, where they entered the brain stem, and the Nucleus magnocellularis became accessible for electrode penetra- tions after removing the bone overlying the cerebellum and aspira- tion of the cerebellum. After surgery, glass micro-electrodes filled with 3 M KC1 and having resistances of 30-80 Mr2 were located close to the ganglion or the nerve, then the birds were placed in an electrically-shielded sound-proof room. To record the neuro- nal response electrodes were advanced into the ganglion or the nerve using a hydraulic drive.

Sound system. Acoustic stimuli were presented via a closed sound system which was fit tightly into the outer ear canal of the bird.

The sound system consisted of a speaker ( A K G D K K 32) and a calibrated measuring microphone (Brfiel & Kjaer 4133) for moni- toring the stimulus near the ear drum. The output of the sound system was flat within +_ 3 dB in a frequeny range between 0.05 and 4 kHz. Tones were generated by a Wavetek-Rockland mod- el 5100 frequency synthesizer; noise stimuli were presented from a prerecorded tape (TEAC 2340SX, tape speed 19 cm/s).

Analysis of a fiber's response to tones. Upon encountering a nerve fibre, the response to a matrix of tone bursts (100 ms, 2.5 ms rise and fall time, rate 4/s) covering a range of frequencies (3 octaves in 0.2 octave steps) around the characteristic frequency (CF, fre- quency of highest sensitivity) and sound pressure levels (generally between 10 and 90 dB SPL varying in 4 dB steps) were measured using an automated procedure. Each combination of frequency and sound pressure was presented twice. Iso-rate contours were calculated from the responses to the tone bursts of the test matrix.

By increasing the rate criterion above spontaneous rate the first ' s m o o t h ' iso-rate curve (generally 40% above the spontaneous ac- tivity) was taken as the tuning curve (Gleich and Narins 1988).

The CF, the threshold at CF, the bandwidth 10 dB above CF- threshold, and the QlodB value (CF/bandwidth) were determined from the tuning curves.

Analysis of a fiber's response to noise stimuli with gaps. Next, the neuron's response to a series of broadband noise stimuli with gaps of various sizes was recorded. The gap-stimuli were composed of two bursts of digitally-generated frozen Gaussian noise (i.e. always generated from the same time series of amplitude values), a leading burst of 800 ms (8 ms rise time at the beginning only) and a trailing burst of 100 ms (8 ms fall time at the end only) separated by a gap (for more details on stimulus generation see Klump and Maier 1989). Rise and fall times at the gap were less than 150 ~ts. The gap stimuli were presented in a series starting with a gap size of 0.4 ms and doubling the gap-size with each new stimulus in the series up to 204.8 ms. A control stimulus that was composed of the two noise bursts, but without a gap between, was presented as the first stimulus of a gap series. The stimuli were repeated at a rate of one gap stimulus per 2.7 s. The whole series was repeat- ed between 8 and 15 times. Sound-pressure levels between 30 and 87 dB SPL were used. Some neurons were tested at more than one SPL.

The statistical analysis of the response followed the procedure given in Buchfellner et al. (1989) for the same stimulus series. Brief- ly, spikes were counted in two types of time windows (TW), one starting at the end of the leading noise burst to measure the neu- ron's OFF-response (Constant TW, durations were 5, 10, 15, 30 ms), and the other starting at the beginning of the trailing noise burst to measure the neuron's ON-response (Variable TW, dura- tions were 5, 10, 15, 30, 100 ms). The starting points of both types of TWs were adjusted for the neuron's latency of the response.

The neuronal activity in the corresponding TWs for the stimulus without a gap was used as the control.

The minimum-detectable gap was determined as follows : if the activity in the corresponding TWs of the 11 different gap stimuli was not uniformly distributed (KS-test), the TWs with spike numbers that differed significantly from the number in response to the control stimulus (no gap) were determined using a binomial test. The smallest gap duration for which the activity significantly (P2 < 0.05) differed between gap stimulus and control was defined as the unit's threshold for the corresponding TW. Additionally, the response to the next larger gap also had to show a significant deviation from the control. The minimum-detectable gap was final- ly defined as the minimum gap threshold of all TWs.

General data analysis. All P-values given are two-tailed. Gap dura- tions were log-transformed before statistical analysis, to ensure a Gaussian distribution of minimum-detectable gaps. Correlation coefficients are Pearson's r or the multivariate correlation coeffi- cient R. All correlations and their P-values given in the text are based on one data-point per neuron at the sound-pressure level that was within a range of SPLs where in the study of behavioural gap thresholds no effect of SPL was found (Klump and Maier 1989). Additionally, this SPL was also the nearest to the one used in the gap-detection study of forebrain neurons (Buchfellner et al.

1989).

Results

S u f f i c i e n t d a t a f o r a n a l y s i s o f b o t h t u n i n g a n d t e m p o r a l c o d i n g w e r e o b t a i n e d f r o m 35 p r i m a r y a u d i t o r y a f f e r - ents. T e n u n i t s w e r e r e c o r d e d in t h e c o c h l e a r g a n g l i o n a n d 25 in t h e V I I I t h n e r v e . R e s p o n s e c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s f r o m u n i t s r e c o r d e d u n d e r t h e s e t w o c o n d i t i o n s w e r e n o t d i f - f e r e n t ; h o w e v e r , u n i t s w i t h h i g h e r C F s w e r e m o r e e a s i l y

(3)

g0

O

60 50 4 0

30 20 10 0

I I 1 I

O 0 0

0 Do [] O []

/

~oo 8~ o

Oo [] Oo [] Oo [] 0

O

l l L I

0.5 1 2 5

cF [kH~.]

Fig. 1. Thresholds of single units at CF in the auditory periphery of the starling and their relation to the starling's behavioural threshold curve (Dooling et al. 1986)

m

0

0 r

15

t O

I I I

[]

[] o

o []

[] %

o

o o

o o

o o []

o o

o o

0 i i i

0,5 t ;2 3

Cl r [kHz]

Fig. 2. Relationship between the CF and the QxodB of single units in the starling's auditory periphery

accessible with the brain-stem approach than with gang- lion recordings (Manley et al. 1985).

CFs, tuning and thresholds

The neuron's CFs ranged from 0.6 to 4 kHz. The lowest threshold was 5 dB SPL at a CF o f 1.3 k H z (Fig. 1), and no threshold at CF was higher than 62 dB. The lowest thresholds o f neurons in this study are close to the behavioural absolute threshold curves of the starling

(Fig. 1). The units' spontaneous discharge rates ranged from 1.9 to 101.3 spikes s - z (mean 28.9 spikes s - z). The sharpness o f tuning as measured by the Q~OdB showed the same average increase with CF (Fig. 2, r = 0.402, P <

0.02) as has been shown before in the starling by Manley et al. (1985). The range o f Q~odB values was 1.45 to 17.

The neurons' coding of gaps

A minimum-detectable gap could be determined in all o f the neurons within the range o f gap durations tested (0.4 to 204.8 ms; in one neuron a minimum-detectable At (ms)

2 0 4 . 8

1 o 2 , ... Lk, . . . .

2 5 . 8 . . . i v . . . . ,

12.8 ... J~J 'L[-ki"Z'J'i'li~h"z' d"LmAL'~ a h~ ... ' ...

6 . 4 ... ~ "l'l'l'kt't'k IL'I~ LataL'ki'' a~'lt~ ...

3.2 ... LL' d"tJ'~l'd'~ "i'"d "dl~at L J ...

1 . 6 . . . ~ a / a d d , ddl,tud,.tJ~ i L~t~u,,[,, . . .

0.8 ... ~,,I l'Llldl''a'l~i'J'il~kl"'zddL'zL'ju~L .,L ,.L~-.

0 . 4 . . . ~t~.,=~,,J ~l,~J~z .t,..iLj.u~j.~ L ...

i

5 0

200ms

Fig. 3. An example of the coding of gaps by an 8th nerve fibre (the peri-stimulus time histograms have a bin-width of 10 ms; the arrow indicates where the gap starts). Gaps can be coded either by response decrement at the end of the leading noise burst (from 12.8 ms gap size on in this recording), or by the ON-excitation at the beginning of the noise burst trailing the gap (the phasic response component is detectable at a gap size of 25.6 ms and above)

(4)

472 G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection a

1 0 0

5 O

m~

2 0 L~

;11 5

0

I 0 , 5

0

0

I 0

0 0 0 []

Im O0 0 0 0 DO0 0

0 O0 IlO 0

Q

t~

I I I I

1 2 3 4

C V [ k H z ]

b

t~

1 0 0

5 0

2 0

1 0

5

c

1 0 0

5o

'~ 2 0

.~ 1 0

~ 5

8

i i i

oo

oo m

o o o o o

ooo o im om o

o o a ~ a o

o o

o

I t I t

0 2 4 6

I 0 , 2

I T I I I f I

0

[]

I I I I I 118

8 1 0 1 2 1 4 1 6

Q lOdB

i i

o o

o o o o o o o o o

oo o o 0~ [] []

o o o

o

I I

0 , 5 1

B a n d w i d t h [ k H z ]

Fig. 4 a-e. Size of the minimum-detectable gap in relation to neuro- nal tuning: a minimum-detectable gap vs. CF; b minimum-detect- able gap vs. Q10aa; c minimum-detectable gap vs. bandwidth 10 dB above threshold at CF

g a p c o u l d not be determined at a s t i m u l u s l e v e l of 60 dB SPL, b u t it was measured at both 75 and 87 dB SPL).

If a gap threshold could be determined for a constant TW, it resulted in all cases from the reduction in spike- rate at the end o f the leading noise burst. If a gap thresh- old could be determined for variable TW, it resulted in all cases from an ON-excitation triggered by the be- ginning o f the trailing noise burst (for an example o f a neuron that shows both decrement in spike rate and

a

1 0 0

" ~ 5 0

r~ 20

. ~ 1 0

.i

:i 5

[ I I [ I I

0 []

0 0 0 0

0 0 0 0 0

0 ~ 0 030 [] O

0 0 0 I O I C D 0

O 0 0

o

I I I I I I

0 :80 4 0 8 0 8 0 1 0 0

S p o n t a n e o u s R a t e I f / s ]

b

1 0 0

~ ' 5 O

20

= 1 0

c

1 0 0

~ ' 5 0

2o

. ~ 1 0

,1 5

0 0

I

I 0

[ I I I I I

0 0

0 0 0 0

0 0 0 0 []

0 0 O 0 0 ~ O 0 0

[] 0 0 0 0 0 IOCI

I I I I I

20 30 40 50 60

Theshold [dB]

I I I i i I

o []

o o ~ []

o m o o o o

o o [ : ~ o o o o ~ n ~ o o

o o ~ o o o o []

o [] o

[] o

I I I I I I

- 8 0 0 2 0 4 0 6 0 8 0

S e n s a t i o n L e v e l [ d B ]

Fig. 5a-e. Size of the minimum-detectable gap in relation to the neuron's activity patterns" a minimum-detectable gap vs. spontane- ous rate; b minimum-detectable gap vs. neuronal threshold at CF;

c minimum-detectable gap vs. level of stimulation above the neuro- nal threshold (data points in this figure also include repeat mea- surements of the same fibres at different SPL of the stimulus)

ON-excitation, see Fig. 3). The m i n i m u m - d e t e c t a b l e g a p

resulted significantly more often from gap thresholds defined by reduced spike-rates (66% o f a l l m e a s u r e - m e n t s ) than by gap thresholds defined by O N - e x c i t a -

(5)

G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection 473 tions (20% of all measurements). In only 14% of all

measurements did ON- and OFF-responses result in the same gap threshold.

A stepwise multiple regression analysis of the influ- ence of spontaneous rate, absolute threshold, sound pressure level of stimulation relative to threshold (' sen- sation level'), CF, and Q x OdR on the minimum-detectable gap revealed that the most important factor determining the minimum-detectable gap is the sound-pressure level of stimulation relative to the neuron's threshold (partial correlation coefficient=-0.39, P<0.05, n = 3 5 neu- rons). None of the other variables showed a significant influence on the minimum-detectable gap in this analy- sis. Replacing the QIOdB with the bandwidth in kHz 10 dB above threshold at CF did not yield different re- sults.

None of the bivariate relationships between the inde- pendent variables used in the multiple regression analysis describing the tuning of the neuron and the minimum- detectable gap lead to a significant correlation (Fig. 4).

Both the neuron's threshold and its 'sensation level' show a significant correlation with the minimum detect- able gap (Fig. 5). However, the multivariate correlation coefficient shows that the neuron's thresholds by them- selves do not influence time resolution, and that the sig- nificant bivariate relationship is the result of a correla- tion between the neuron's thresholds and the 'sensation levels' of the presentation of the sound.

Discussion

The relationship between temporal resolution and tuning in peripheral neurons

In general, there is a trade-off in signal-analysis systems between resolution in the frequency domain and the time domain. Because filters with small bandwidths that are needed for good frequency resolution have longer time constants than filters with large bandwidths, time resolu- tion is inversely related to frequency resolution. There- fore, we would expect a negative correlation between the bandwidth of a neuron 10 dB above threshold and its minimum-detectable gap (which can be considered as a measure of a neuron's time resolution). By similar argument, a positive correlation between the sharpness of tuning measured by a neuron's Q1OdB and its mini- mum detectable gap is to be expected. We found neither correlation in our study, and furthermore the signs of the non-significant correlation coefficients that we re- port here are not consistent with the theoretical expecta- tion.

Viemeister (1979) proposed a model consisting of a bandpass filter followed by a half-wave rectifier and a lowpass filter to explain human psychophysical data on temporal resolution. The output of this model was moni- tored by a decision mechanism analyzing the variance to detect changes in the amplitude of a broadband noise signal passed through the model. Green and Forrest (1988) used the same model, but with a different decision mechanism (i.e. a detector performing a comparison of

the maximum and the minimum amplitude within a cer- tain time window), to explain human gap detection and temporal modulation transfer function data. Their cri- teria were equivalent to those used in our single cell study on gap detection. For a bandwidth of 4000 Hz for the bandpass filter and a time constant of 3 ms for the lowpass filter, their model resulted in a good fit to the measured human gap detection and modulation transfer function data. The bandwidth of 400 Hz and above and the time constants between 1.5 and 12 ms used in their simulations and model resulted in a weaker influence of bandwidth on temporal resolution than would be expected using a simple inverse relation be- tween bandwidth and time resolution (de Boer 1985).

This is in accord with our findings on the starling's pe- ripheral neurons ability to detect gaps. As Patterson (1988) pointed out, however, the bandwidth necessary to give a good fit to the psychoacoustic data using Green's and Forrest's model does not reflect the band- width of the human's peripheral auditory filters (already Viemeister 1979 discussed this discrepancy for his formu- lation of the model). Also the starling's peripheral neu- rons show bandwidths which in general are an order of magnitude smaller than those assumed for the models discussed above (Manley et al. 1985; this study).

Klump and Maier (1989) estimated the starling's pe- ripheral time constants using data from Manley et al.

(1985) on peripheral tuning and de Boer's (1985) theoret- ical estimate of the time constant of peripheral auditory filters. They conclude that in the starling's peripheral auditory system time constants may only be limiting for time resolution at frequencies below 1 to 2 kHz. How- ever, considering only the bandwidth as an effector of temporal resolution is probably too simplistic. The work by de Boer (1985) and de Boer and Kruidenier (1990) indicate that the temporal resolution decreases with in- creasing order of a filter for a given bandwidth. The high- and low-frequency slopes of tuning curves from starling auditory afferents that can exceed 200 dB/octave (Manley et al. 1985) are obviously the result of compli- cated higher order filter systems and thus their temporal resolution tends to be overestimated if it is only based on filter bandwidth. Furthermore, the large variability of tuning curve slopes and the increase in the tuning curve slopes with increasing CF (Manley et al. 1985) will lead to a large variation in the unit's time constants and may thus obscure a relationship between bandwidth and minimum detectable gap. Since it was suggested that the tuning mechanisms in the starling cochlea differs for low (< 1 kHz) compared to high (> 1 kHz) CF-re- gions (Gleich 1989; Manley 1986), a resolution of this issue requires a large amount of data from a restricted frequency region which currently is not available. In con- trast to our results in the starling, Dunia and Narins (1989), report a correlation between the minimum time constant measured for phase locking to the sinusoidal amplitude modulations of a broadband carrier and QxodB for frog auditory afferents. However, this correla- tion was quite weak and accounts for only 5% of the variance in the minimum integration time.

Apparently conflicting psychophysical results be-

(6)

474 G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection

tween a measure of frequency selectivity (i.e. critical bandwidth) and a measure of temporal resolution (i.e.

gap-detection threshold for bandlimited signals) have been described in the budgerigar (Okanoya and Dooling 1990). In this species temporal resolution does not corre- late with predictions generated from the measures of frequency resolution. At 3 kHz, where frequency selec- tivity is very high, the minimum detectable gap is about 3 ms, although even conservative estimates would pre- dict values above 5 ms (Okanoya and Dooling 1990;

de Boer 1985). This discrepancy might be explained, however, if the budgerigar's auditory afferents show sim- ilar variability in tuning as reported for other birds and use different subpopulations of neurons for different tasks.

The relationship between temporal resolution and the neuron's activity pattern

Peripheral neurons in the starling show no correlation between their spontaneous discharge rate and the abso- lute threshold at the neuron's CF (Manley et al. 1985;

this study). Similarly, the precision of the coding of time patterns seems not to correlate with the neuron's sponta- neous rate (see Fig. 5 a). This suggests that neither the factors responsible for the variation in spontaneous ac- tivity nor the spontaneous activity itself influence the coding of stimulus intensity or its changes. In the star- ling's forebrain, however, Buchfellner (1987) described a correlation between the size of the minimum-detectable gap and the spontaneous activity. The mean spontane- ous activity of the neurons in her study, which also in-

peripheral neurons

t2 T a

8 6 4

2 o

z ar li f r i r

,o

0.4 0.8 1.6 3.2 5.4 12.8 25.6 51.2 102.4 204.8 >204.8

Minimum Gop [ms]

Fig. 6a, b. Distribution of minimum-detectable gaps in the periph- ery of the auditory system (a) and of minimum-detectable gaps in primary-like neurons in the starling's auditory forebrain (b, data from Buchfellner 1987)

cluded different response types other than primary-like, was only half than that found in the auditory periphery of the starling; and the neurons coding the shortest gaps had an average spontaneous activity that was similar to the average spontaneous activity of the starling's pe- ripheral neurons.

The factors influencing the driven activity of a star- ling's peripheral neuron, or the driven activity itself, seem to have an impact on the neuron's ability to code rapid changes in stimulus intensity with time (i.e. such as short gaps). The threshold at CF itself and the level of stimulation above the neuron's threshold seem to cor- relate with the size of the minimum-detectable gap. How- ever, a multiple regression analysis revealed that the level of stimulation correlates better than the threshold itself (which by definition is correlated with the level of stimu- lation). If the effect of level of stimulation is removed from the regression equation, the threshold at the CF itself no longer correlates with the minimum detectable gap. This result is comparable to findings of Smith (1977, 1979) and by Harris and Dallos (1979) who studied neu- ral adaptation and its effect on forward masking in the auditory nerve of the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones ungui- culatus) and the Chinchilla (Chinchilla laniger). Smith (1979) suggests that the strength of a neuron's response during the stimuli is a very strong determinant of its response towards a probe stimulus presented following a silent gap after a masker in a forward-masking para- digm. Furthermore, the decrement in spike-rate after the offset of a stimulus depends on the activity elicited by it. Since both the decrement and ON-activation are the two responses by which a gap is coded, the effects of stimulus level that influence the amount of forward masking will also influence gap-detection by peripheral neurons.

Temporal resolution of neurons at different levels of the starling's auditory system and the psychophysics of gap detection

Both in the starling (forebrain neurons, Buchfellner et al.

1989) and in the acridid grasshopper (interneuron AN4, Ronacher and Stumpner 1988) the time resolution pro- vided by the peripheral auditory system is preserved in the auditory pathway (see peripheral and central distri- bution of minimum detectable gaps in Fig. 6). The medi- an minimum-detectable gap of primary-like neurons in the starling's auditory forebrain (12.8 ms for stimuli with a sound pressure level of 55 dB SPL) is equal to the median minimum-detectable gap found in primary fibres in the peripheral auditory system for sound pressure lev- els of 55 to 70 dB SPL. Thus, integration of temporal information across many neurons in the auditory path- way is not needed to achieve the high temporal resolu- tion found in a recent behavioural study (the psychoa- coustic measure of the minimum-detectable gap was 1.8 ms; Klump and Maier 1989). The peripheral neurons with the lowest gap detection thresholds (below 6.4 ms) would adequately transmit the information on the tem- poral pattern with a resolution necessary to account for

(7)

G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection 475 the psychoacoustic thresholds. The effect o f sensation

level on the size o f the minimum-detectable gap in the psychoacoustic study is paralleled by our finding that the a m o u n t o f stimulation relative to a peripheral neu- r o n ' s threshold (i.e. the n e u r o n ' s ' s e n s a t i o n level') is a d e t e r m i n a n t o f the ability to detect short gaps.

The fact that in the starling's peripheral neurons a decrease in spike rate is a better indicator o f the presence o f a gap, whereas in the starling's auditory forebrain an O N - r e s p o n s e elicited by the noise burst following the g a p is the better indicator o f a gap suggests that there m a y be a change in the neuronal encoding o f tem- poral patterns between the peripheral and the central auditory system. It has been shown in studies on the encoding o f sinusoidal amplitude m o d u l a t i o n in toads (Rose and C a p r a n i c a 1985) and m a m m a l s (see review in Rees and Palmer 1989) that in the peripheral auditory system, a synchronization p a t t e r n m o r e precisely en- codes the fluctuations in the signal envelope t h a n an intensity-rate-code, whereas m o r e centrally it is the re- verse. The change in the way the minimum-detectable gap is encoded in the starling's auditory system suggests a similar transition in codes.

The similarity of the limits of peripheral temporal resolution in gap detection tasks in different animals The observation that the study o f the detection o f short intensity decrements in b r o a d b a n d noise results in the same m i n i m u m - d e t e c t a b l e gap in m a n y different taxa suggests that it is based on very c o m m o n neural mecha- nisms. R o n a c h e r and R t m e r (1985) showed that single p r i m a r y receptor cells of the hearing organ o f an acridid g r a s s h o p p e r (Chorthippus biguttulus) can code m i n i m u m gap sizes o f a b o u t 2 ms. The study o f acoustical p r i m a r y receptor cells in two species o f noctuid m o t h s (Agrotis segetum and Noctua pronuba) by Surlykke et al. (1988) showed a similar minimum-detectable gap o f 2 ms.

Z h a n g et al. (1990) found minimum-detectable gaps o f below 2 ms in the chinchilla (Chinchilla laniger), and also in this study the shortest minimum-detectable gaps in the starling are in the range o f 1.6 to 3.2 ms. In all these examples, the size o f the m i n i m u m - d e t e c t a b l e gap was either defined by the difference between the spike rate at the onset o f the noise-burst following the gap and the reduced spike rate during the gap, or by the decre- m e n t o f the steady-state rate during the ongoing noise stimulus to rates close to zero after the end o f noise burst preceding the gap.

The only species in which an unusually low capacity for coding t e m p o r a l gaps in b r o a d b a n d noise has been found is the goldfish (Fay 1985). Saccular nerve fibres o f the goldfish have little capacity for coding very short gaps by spike-rate decrements because o f their large vari- ability in spike rate over time. Fay suggested that only the increment in spike rate at the onset o f the second noise pulse ending the gap m a y be analyzed by the gold- fish's auditory system. However, an increase in this spike rate requires a relatively large size o f the gap. The low capacity o f the goldfish to code noise energy decrements

is also obvious in the size o f its psychoacoustically-deter- mined minimum-detectable gap o f 35 ms (Fay 1985).

Acknowledgements. We thank R.J. Dooling, G.A. Manley, G.

Neuweiler and H. Wagner for their comments on the earlier version of the manuscript. G.A. Manley also helped us develop the VIIIth nerve approach. A. Kthler prepared the figures. The study was funded by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 204 to GMK and to G.A. Manley).

References

Boer E de (1985) Auditory time constants: a paradox? In: Michel- sen A (ed) Time resolution in auditory systems. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 141-158

Boer E de, Kruidenier C (1990) On ringing limits of the auditory periphery. Biol Cybern 63 : 433-442

Buchfellner E (1987) Untersuchungen zur Kodierung von Pausen in WeiBem Rauschen durch Neurone des caudalen Telencepha- Ion des Staren (Sturnus vulgaris). Diplom thesis, Zoology Dept Technical University Munich

Buchfellner E, Leppelsack H-J, Klump GM, H/iusler U (1989) Gap detection in the starling (Sturnus vulgaris): II. Coding of gaps by forebrain neurons. J Comp Physiol A 164:539-549

Dooling RJ, Okanoya K, Downing J, Hulse S (1986) Hearing in the starling (Sturnus vulgaris): Absolute thresholds and critical ratios. Bull Psychonom Soc 24:462-466

Dunia R, Narins PM (1989) Temporal resolution in frog auditory- nerve fibers. J Acoust Soc Am 82:1630-1638

Fay RR (1985) Sound intensity processing by the goldfish. J Acoust Soc Am 78:1296-1309

Giraudi D, Salvi R, Henderson D, Hamerik R (1980) Gap detection by the chinchilla. J Acoust Soc Am 68 : 802-806

Gleich O (1989) Auditory primary afferents in the starling: Correla- tion of function and morphology. Hearing Res 37: 255-268 Gleich O, Narins PM (1988) The phase response of primary audito-

ry afferents in a songbird (Sturnus vulgaris L.). Hearing Res 32:81-92

Green DM, Forrest TG (1988) Detection of amplitude modulation and gaps in noise. In: Duifhuis H, Horst JW, Witt HP (eds) Basic issues in hearing. Academic Press, London, pp 323-330 Harris DM, Dallos P (1979) Forward masking of auditory nerve

fibre responses. J Neurophysiol 42:1083-1107

Helversen D von (1972) Gesang des M/innchens und Lautschema des Weibchens bei der Feldheuschrecke Chorthippus biguttulus (Orthoptera, Acrididae). J Comp Physiol 81:381-422

Klump GM, Maier EH (1989) Gap detection in the starling (Stur- nus vulgaris): I. Psychophysical thresholds. J Comp Physiol A 164:531-539

Manley GA (1986) The evolution of the mechanisms of frequency selectivity in vertebrates. In: Moore BCJ, Patterson RD (eds) Auditory frequency selectivity. Plenum, New York London, pp 63-72

Manley GA, Gleich O, Leppelsack H-J, Oeckinghaus H (1985) Activity patterns of cochlear ganglion neurons in the starling.

J Comp Physiol A 157:161-181

Okanoya K, Dooling RJ (1990) Minimum detectable gap in noise as a function of intensity and frequency for two avian species, budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) and zebra finches (Poe- phila guttata). Hearing Res 50:185-192

Patterson RD (1988) Comment on Green DM, Forrest TG: Detec- tion of amplitude modulation and gaps in noise. In: Duifhuis H, Horst JW, Witt HP (eds) Basic issues in hearing. Academic Press, London, p 331

Rees A, Palmer AR (1989) Neuronal responses to amplitude-modu- lated and pure-tone stimuli in the guinea pig inferior colliculus, and their modification by broadband noise. J Acoust Soc Am 85:1978-1994

Ronacher B, Rtmer H (1985) Spike synchronization of tympanic

(8)

476 G.M. Klump and O. Gleich: Starling peripheral gap detection receptor fibres in a grasshopper (Chorthippus biguttulus L., Ac-

rididae). A possible mechanism for the detection of gaps in model songs. J Comp Physiol A 157:631-642

Ronacher B, Stumpner A (1988) Filtering of behaviourally relevant temporal parameters of a grasshopper's song by an auditory interneuron. J Comp Physiol A 163:517-523

Rose G, Capranica R (1985) Sensitivity to amplitude modulated sounds in the anuran auditory nervous system. J Neurophysiol 53 : 446-465

Salvi R J, Arehole S (1985) Gap detection in chinchillas with tempo- rary high-frequency hearing loss. J Acoust Soc Am 77:1173 1177

Shailer MJ, Moore BCJ (1983) Gap detection as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level. J Acoust Soc Am 74:467-473

Smith RL (1977) Short-term adaptation in single auditory nerve fibres: some poststimulatory effects. J Neurophysiol 40:1098- 1112

Smith RL (1979) Adaptation, saturation, and physiological mask- ing in single auditory nerve fibres. J Acoust Soe Am 65 : 166-178 Surlykke A, Larsen O, Michelsen A (1988) Temporal coding in the auditory receptor of the moth ear. J Comp Physiol A 162: 367-374

Viemeister NF (1979) Temporal modulation transfer functions based upon modulation thresholds. J Aeoust Soc Am 66 : 1364- 1380

Zhang W, Salvi RJ, Saunders SS (1990) Neural correlates of gap detection in auditory-nerve fibers of the chinchilla. Hearing Res, 46:181-200

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

Frequency of people who currently belong to a religious denomination On average, 64.9% of adult EU citizens belong to a religious denomination.. Own calculation

Therefore, we introduce the so called dual coding technique, which provides two main advantages over using classical matrix codes: First of all, a higher spatial information density

On a population level neurons of the two tracts, the m- and the l- ACT, have overlapping response profiles, excluding the possibility that the separation of olfactory receptor

(2003) Changes in spontaneous firing rate and neural synchrony in cat primary auditory cortex after localized tone-induced hearing loss. and

odor response profile of a Drosophila melanogaster Meigen (Diptera: Drosophilidae) olfactory receptor; furthermore, we will review data from the honeybee antennal lobe (AL),

It is expected that the present study results will allow, among other things, to design strategies to obtain the support of the citizenship for the conservation of wildlife

We use secondary data for 2015 and 2017 from Jaminan Kesehatan Nasional (JKN), a patient level dataset for Indonesia created in 2014 to record public and private hospitals’ claims

Comparing these results with the dynamical population response properties of real neurons, I showed that the generalized θ-neuron exhibits a similar response behavior: For both