• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Heritability and adaptive significance of the number of egg-dummies in the cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Heritability and adaptive significance of the number of egg-dummies in the cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni"

Copied!
7
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Heritability and adaptive significance of the number of egg-dummies in the cichlid

fish Astatotilapia burtoni

Topi K. Lehtonen

1,2

and Axel Meyer

1

,*

1 Lehrstuhl fur Zoologie und Evolutionsbiologie, Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany

2Section of Ecology, Department of Biology, University of Turku, 20014 Turku, Finland

Cichlid fishes are a textbook example of rapid speciation and exuberant diversity-this applies especially to haplochromines, a lineage with approximately 1800 species. Haplochromine males uniquely possess oval, bright spots on their anal fin, called 'egg-spots' or 'egg-dummies'. These are presumed to be an evol- utionary key innovation that contributed to the tribe's evolutionary success. Egg-spots have been proposed to mimic the ova of the mouthbrooding females of the corresponding species, contribute to fer- tilization success and even facilitate species recognition. Interestingly, egg-spot number varies extensively not only between species, but also within some populations. This high degree of intraspecific variation may appear to be counterintuitive since selection might be expected to act to stabilize traits that are cor- related with fitness measures. We addressed this 'paradox' experimentally, and found that in the haplochromine cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni, the number of egg-spots was related to male age, body con- dition and dominance status. Intriguingly, the egg-spot number also had a high heritable component (narrow sense heritability of O.S). These results suggest that the function of egg-spots might have less to do with fertilization success or species recognition, but rather relate to mate choice and/or male- male competition, helping to explain the high variability in this important trait.

Keywords: body condition; dominance hierarchy; narrow sense heritability; intraspecific variation;

key innovation; signal value

1. INTRODUCTION

A famously high number of species, and unsurpassed rates of evolution, have made cichlid fishes of the great East African lakes one of the most remarkable examples of adaptive radiation [1-4]. The great majority of these species (approximately 1800 of the 2S00 species in the family Cichlidae) belong to a single lineage, the haplo- chromine cichlids [S]. The anal fin of a sexually mature haplochromine male carries bright yellow or orange ovoid spots, which have been suggested to mimic real eggs of the species, and have therefore been called egg- dummies [6]. Females of some species also have spots, but these are less pronounced and on average lower in number. Interestingly, the less diverse lineages of cichlids do not posses similar egg-spots [7], and indeed, posses- sion of egg-dummies may have promoted speciation within the group [S,8] and have been considered to be a key innovation that has contributed to the evolutionary success of haplochromines [S,7]. As a consequence of the particular mating system of female mouthbrooding and the concomitant evolution of egg-spots, these fish are limited by the number of eggs that fit into a female's mouth (typically only 20-40), which might have contrib- uted to smaller populations sizes and hence faster rates of evolution [S,7].

* Author for correspondence (axel.meyer@uni-konstanz.de).

In haplochromines, fertilization typically takes place in the mouth of the female after she has nipped at the male's egg-dummies, which are located near the male's genital opening (figure 1; see the electronic supplementary material, video clip; see also [7]). Hence, egg-spots have been suggested to enhance fertilization success and facili- tate spawning ([6], but see [9]). A few other functions for the spots have also been proposed (e.g. [8,10]), such as certainty of paternity and species recognition. Indeed, arrangement and number of egg-spots differs between haplochromine species [7,10-12].

Interestingly, in some species the number of egg-spots is not fixed, but varies among individuals of a population.

For example, in a natural breeding colony of A. burtoni, the number of egg-spots ranged between five and nine [13]. Similarly, sibling males of a Lake Malawi haplochro- mine species (Labeotropheus) were reported to have from two to six egg-spots [14]. This type of variation calls for an explanation, as traits important for successful fertili- zation, species recognition or fitness, in general, are expected to be under stabilizing selection, which should erode heritable genetic variation behind these traits (e.g.

[IS]). However, so far the sources for this variation in egg-spots, an evolutionary key trait [S], have not been investigated.

We examined within-species variation in the number of egg-spots in the haplochromine cichlid A. burtoni, a widely used model system in behavioural endocrinology and genomics [16-22]. The species lives in ponds and rIvers around Lake Tanganyika, East Africa. As First publ. in: Proceedings of the Royal Society B 278 (2011), 1716, pp. 2318-2324

doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2483

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URN: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-127725

(2)

10cm

Figure 1. Male A. burtoni and a close-up of his anal fin with egg-spots. A 1 mm grid on the background provides a scale for assessing the size of the egg-spots.

characteristic for haplochromines, females of the species are mouth brooders, whereas males do not contribute to the care of offspring. Males are highly aggressive and only successful territory holders are able to attract females to spawn [13]. A manipulation of egg-spot number in a haplochromine species closely related to A. burtoni suggested that females discriminate against males with a reduced number of spots [9]. This is notable since the number of egg-spots is highly variable among males of the species (among males used in this study 4- 15; see also [11,13]). We tested five mutually non-exclusive explanations that could contribute to the high variation in egg-spot number, observed within A. burtoni popu- lations: (1) egg-spot number is a function of body size

[11], e.g. because large males also have larger anal fins that can potentially accommodate a larger number of egg-dummies of a given size, (2) the number of egg- dummies increases over time [14], which could predict the number of spots better than body size per se, (3) the number of egg-spots depends on body condition of the male, suggesting that they are costly to produce or main- tain [23,24], (4) the number of egg-dummies is a badge of status, or more generally, dominant males have more spots [14,25], and finally, (5) explanations 3 and 4 include the possibility that the number of spots has a sig- nificant genetic component and it may therefore signal heritable differences between the males that selection can act on. We designed a series oflaboratory experiments to test each of these possibilities.

2. MATERIAL AND METHODS

The experiments were conducted in the animal care facility at the University of Konstanz, Germany, under a 12 L: 12 D cycle and water temperature of 25

±

2°e. The fish were fed daily with commercial fish foods if not otherwise noted. Juven- iles were also fed with newly hatched brine shrimps (Artemia).

Under these conditions, A. burtoni reaches sexual maturity and starts to breed at the age of approximately four months, with even the smallest and most subdominant individuals

maturing in six months (T. K. Lehtonen & A. Meyer, 2008, personal observations), having attained a total length of 6-6.5 cm ([26]; T. K. Lehtonen & A. Meyer, 2008, personal observations). In the wild, the exact population structure seems to depend on the habitat [26,27], but most breeding populations are likely to consist of young individuals due to

heavy natural predation and fishery [26,27]. Under these con- ditions, males do not seem to survive long enough to exceed the total length of 10-12 cm [26], although in captivity the species can ultimately attain a larger size (15 cm or even larger) and live for several years. However, many individuals start to show signs of senescence (ragged fins, deformations, increased mortality and decreased rate of breeding) even before the age of 2 years (T. K. Lehtonen & A. Meyer, 2008, personal observations). Accordingly, we used young, mature males to assess the following five explanations for the high within-population variation in the number of egg-spots in male A. burtoni.

(a) Explanation 1: body size

To assess whether there is a relationship between male size and the number or size of his egg-spots, we photographed four different groups of males: (1) 64 males of the age of 7.5 months (the second generation from 'explanation 5', see below), (2) 12 relatively young, mature males (age 10.5 months), (3) another of group of males of the same age (10.5 months, n = 15) and (4) 12 older, large males (16 months). At least two photographs of each male were taken on a grid paper that was covered with a smooth, transparent plastic sheet. Two photographs of each male were later ana- lysed using Sigma Scan Pro 5.0 (SPSS Inc.) software, and average values from these measurements were used for stat- istical analyses. Where relevant, this same photography procedure was also used in other experiments, described below. We counted as an egg-spot a bright, ovoid-shaped spot on the male's anal fin with a rim of lighter coloration around it (figure 1, see also [7]).

(b) Explanation 2: changes over time

Different stocking conditions (most importantly tank size, com- petitive regime and social background) of the different (age) groups of 'explanation l' did not allow formal tests over these groups. Hence, in order to investigate whether the number (or size) of egg-spots changes over time (and hence presumably with age), males of the groups 2-4 were photographed again after a period of time. Specifically, we kept the males of group 2 singly for two months in tanks measuring 50 x 25 x 30 cm (length x width x height), the males of group 3 in con- tact with 2-4 conspecifics in tanks measuring 60 x 40 x 35 cm for 1-2 months, and the males of group 4 singly for three months in the 50 x 25 x 30 cm tanks.

(c) Explanation 3: condition dependence

In this experiment, we examined whether the number or size of egg-spots depends on body condition of the male. Imma- ture juveniles that did not yet possess egg-spots (age under three months) were divided into two treatments: food restricted individuals (n = 18) were given food on 3 days a week, whereas control individuals were fed daily (n = 18).

The same number (1-5) of individuals from anyone family was used for both treatments. All male offspring (n = 10 in both treatments) were photographed at the age of7.5 months.

Besides comparing egg-spot number and size directly between the treatments, we also ran a general linear model (GLM) on egg-spot number, with total length as a

(3)

Table 1. Total lengths, egg-spot numbers and correlations between these two traits in four experimental groups of A. burtoni males.

total length (mm) male group age (months) mean

±

s.e. range

1 7.5 81

±

0.7 70-100

2 10.5 86

±

1.2 78-95

3 10.5 91

±

1.3 80-98

4 16 114

±

1.3 106-121

continuous factor and treatment as a categorical factor. After the interaction between variables 'total length' and 'treat- ment' was found to be non-significant, we removed it from the model and proceeded with assessing the main effects.

(d) Explanation 4: dominance hierarchy

Earlier work on A. burtoni has shown that dominant, territor- ial males have higher circulating plasma concentrations of androgens than subdominant, non-territorial males [17], but males are nevertheless able to switch between the two territorial stages within minutes depending on the immediate social environment [16]. We had two different set-ups to test for any relationship between male dominance and the number or size of his egg-spots. First, the most dominant male in 14 different family groups (see 'explanation 5', below) was identified (as distinguished by coloration and interactions among the individuals in the tank) and com- pared with the rest of the males of that family. In one case we took an average over three males that appeared to be equally dominant. The sample size for this measurement was 13 because one of the 14 families had only a single male. Secondly, in a separate experiment, two visually size- matched (for length and condition) males (age 16 months) were placed in a tank measuring 100 x 40 x 40 cm. The tank contained a single 20 cm long and 8 cm wide clay arch as a shelter. Behaviour of the males was monitored to assess their relative dominance positions and also to abort the replicate before males were able to cause unnecessary stress or any physical damage to each other. In all 20 repli- cates, only one of the two males claimed the shelter and the same male was also behaviourally dominant in behaviour- al interactions observed between the two males. The males were photographed after the experiment.

(e) Explanation 5: heritable differences

To estimate narrow-sense heritability (h2) of the number and size of egg-spots, 14 males aged 10.5 months were each placed into one tank with three females. At least one of the three females was considered ready to spawn, and the role of the two other was to diffuse aggression. As soon as a female was mouthbrooding, the rest of them were returned to stock tanks and the male was used for the group 3 of 'explanations 1 and 2'. When the juveniles started to swim freely, the female was removed from the tank. Each batch of juveniles (families) was raised in equal-sized partitions measuring 40 x 60 x 50 cm. One large tank (120 x 60 x 50 cm) contained three such enclosures, partitioned with opaque dividers. The enclosure for each batch of juveniles was assigned randomly. After three months, the maximum number of juveniles in each family was limited to 14 by ran- domly removing any number of individuals exceeding 14, to assure as homogenized conditions across the families as

egg-spots (n)

mean

±

s.e. range n r p

7.2

±

0.2 5-11 64 0.265 0.034

6.4

±

0.3 5-8 12 0.331 0.29

9.5

±

0.6 6-14 15 -0.234 0.40 10.3

±

0.8 7-14 12 -0.173 0.59

possible. At the age of 7.5 months, all remammg male offspring (1-14 per family, average 4.6) were photographed.

Narrow-sense heritability (h2) measures the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by additive genetic variance; a high value of h2 indicates a high degree of additive genetic influence. To yield an estimate for h2, and its standard error, the slope of father-male offspring regression analysis and its standard error were multiplied by 2 [28]. In the regression, each offspring value consisted of the mean over the family. All statistical tests were conducted using Systat 12.0 (SPSS Inc.) software.

3. RESULTS

(a) Explanation I: body size

Despite the large range in the egg-spot number and total length among males of each group (table 1), these two variables correlated only within one of the four male groups (table 1). In none of the groups male total length and egg-spot size (area) correlated significantly (p> 0.10 in all cases).

(b) Explanation 2: changes over titne

All values below are given as 'mean ± standard error of the mean', if not otherwise noted. In all cases, males developed 0-2 new egg-spots during the experimental period and none of them lost any. The change in average spot number was 0.7S ± 0.18 (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, z = 2.71, n = 12, P = 0.007), 0.67 ± 0.21 (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, z=2.43, n= IS, p=O.OIS) and from 0.42 ± 0.19 (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, z 1.89, n = 12, P

=

0.OS9) for the groups 2, 3 and 4, respectively (figure 2). The corresponding changes in the size of the spots were 0.40 ± 0.14 mm2 (paired t-test, t = 2.81, d.f. 11, P 0.017), 0.00S±0.12mm2 (paired t-test, t=0.040, d.f.=14, p=0.97) and -0.031±

0.12 mm2 (paired t-test, t = 0.260, d.f. 11, P = 0.80).

Young males also grew during the study period (group 2: paired t-test, t = 8.24, d.f.

=

11, P < 0.001;

group 3: paired t-test, t = 6.11, d.f.

=

14, P < 0.001), whereas there was only a marginally non-significant tendency for older males to gain length during the study period (group 4: paired t-test, t

=

2.18, d.f.

=

11, P

=

0.OS2).

(c) Explanation 3: condition dependence

Our food restriction treatment was effective in manipulat- ing condition: well-fed males were longer (two sample t-test, t

=

3.16, d.f.

=

18, P = O.OOS) and visually clearly more 'bulky' than food-restricted males of the same age.

Furthermore, at the age of 7.S months, well-fed males (6.2 ± 0.9) had on average a larger number of egg-spots than food-restricted males (4.9 ± 0.9) (two sample

(4)

,,;

:.i

1.0

+1 0.8

..0 t5

<::

§ 0.6 '0 co.

'"

g20.4

'-'

.S

0)

a

J 0.2

'"

~

O~~ ____ - L _ _ ~ _ _ _ _ - L _ _ - L _ _ _ _ ~ __

group 2 group 3 group 4

Figure 2. The number of egg-spots at the beginning of a study period subtracted from the egg-spot number at the end of the period. The numbers of egg-spots have increased over time in all instances.

t-test, t = 3.40, d.f. = 18, P

=

0.003). However, overall length differences among these 20 males seemed to over- rule the effect of experimental treatment on egg-spot number (GLM, treatment effect: F],]? = 2.46, P = 0.14;

body size effect: FI,I? 4.59, P = 0.047). The size of egg-spots did not differ between the males of the two treatments (two sample t-test, t

=

0.848, d.f.

=

18, P 0.41).

(d) Explanation 4: dominance hierarchy

In the groups of individuals raised together until the age of 7.5 months, the most dominant males were always larger than the average of the rest of the males (paired t-test, t

=

4.63, d.f.= 12, P = 0.001). However, the number (7.3

±

0.4) or size (2.0

±

0.1 mm2) of egg- spots of these dominant males did not significantly differ from the rest of the males (number: 7.0

±

0.3, paired t-test, t = 0.898, d.f. 12, P = 0.39, figure 3;

size: 1.9

±

0.1 mm2, paired t-test, t = 0.683, d.f. = 12, P 0.51). Nevertheless, among visually size-matched male pairs, the male that exhibited dominant behaviour and acclaimed the single shelter (which as the only avail- able structure typically provided the centre for the new territory) had more egg-spots (10.0

±

0.8) than the sub- dominant male (7.9

±

0.4) (paired t-test, t = 2.57, d.f. = 19, P = 0.019; figure 3). The average size of egg-spots was not different between dominant and sub- dominant males (dominant males: 2.1

±

0.1 mmz;

sub dominant males 2.1

±

0.1 mm2; paired t-test, t

=

0.284, d.f. 19, P = 0.78). The small size differences (0-6.5 mm in total length) between the males in a pair, due to imperfect visual matching, did not affect dominance (dominant male: 109

±

1.4 mm; subdomi- nant: 109

±

1.5 mm; paired t-test, t

=

0.040, d.f.

=

19, P = 0.97).

For the two set-ups combined, dominant males were larger than sub dominants in 24 cases out of 33 (binomial distribution: p = 0.014), had more egg-spots in 18 cases out of 25 (with eight cases of equal egg-spot number, binomial distribution: p = 0.043), and had larger average spot size in 17 cases out of 33 (binomial distribution: p 1.0). Hence, it seems that when there are large size differ- ences between males, body size is the most important

3.0 ,,;

~ 2.5

...

OJ ..0 8 :::; 2.0

c:

'0 1.5

f

cO co 1.0

<l.)

.5

<l.)

0.5

u c:

<l.)

....

<E 0

4-<

I I

'3

--0.5 family groups size-matched males Figure 3. The number of egg-spots of the sub dominant male(s) subtracted from the egg-spot number of the domi- nant male. Positive values indicate that the males with more egg-spots dominated males with fewer spots.

determinant of dominance hierarchy, with egg- spot number being a better predictor among males of approximately similar size.

(e) Explanation S: heritable differences

The number of egg-spots was significantly heritable (h2 = 0.50

±

0.20); males that had many egg-spots (atthe age of 10.5 months) tended to have sons with many egg-spots (figure 4). The estimates for narrow sense heritability of egg-spot size (0.20

±

0.51) and male total length (- 0.32

±

0.44) did not significantly differ from zero.

4. DISCUSSION

It has been reasoned that the evolution of the egg-spots and concomitant evolution of the maternal mouthbrood- ing mating system might have been a key innovation that permitted the evolution of extremely species-rich cichlid fish lineage, the haplochromines, which is composed of about 1800 species [5]. In this study, we set out to explore the sources of intra-population variation in this key trait.

We found, among A. burtoni males of a certain age, mixed evidence regarding the relationship between the number of egg-dummies and body size. We propose that age (Henning & Meyer, 2009, unpublished manuscript) or social background of the males may determine whether such a link between the two traits exists. Heterogeneous male ages may also explain why an earlier study suggests the traits to correlate [11]. In any case, the results of the current study indicate that egg-spot number is not unequivocally determined by body size, especially among males of a certain age. Furthermore, males devel- oped additional egg-spots over time and males in good condition had more spots than low condition males.

Taken together, these findings suggest that egg-spot number has potential to purport somewhat different information than male size per se, giving the female haplo- chromine cichlids the potential to use egg-spot number as a mate choice cue. By preferring males that have high- number of egg-spots, females could mate with older males (for their size) or males that are in good condition.

In some systems, age correlates with a male's survival skills and high immune resistance [29], and females would therefore benefit by choosing old males that pass

(5)

CD c:

'<:;

,v:, 0.

c 0 0

'"

B

10

9

8

7

f f

6

5

4+---.----r---.---,----r---~--._--~

6 7 8 9 10

father

11 12 13 14

Figure 4. The relationship between the number of egg-spots of fathers and their sons based on mean offspring values s.e.) over 14 families.

their good genes to their offspring. Similarly, mating with a male in good condition is generally thought to provide corresponding indirect (and sometimes direct) benefits [23,24,30]. Indeed, females of some haplochromine cichlid species have been reported to use egg-spot number as a mate choice cue [9,31,32], although this is not necessarily the case in A. burtoni (Henning &

Meyer, 2009, unpublished manuscript) or some other haplochromine species [10]. Furthermore, condition dependence of a trait (here egg-spot number) as such may also help to explain how genetic variability in that trait persists ([23,30], see the discussion below).

We also found that the number of egg-spots has rel- evance for the dominance hierarchy: although larger and heavier males generally tend to be more dominant ([10,33]; T. K. Lehtonen & A. Meyer, 2008, personal observations), among males of approximately the same size, individuals with many spots dominated those with a lower egg-spot number. However, in sister groups with high variation in body size, largest males, more often than those with the highest egg-spot number, were socially dominant. Nevertheless, our results suggest that besides potential relevance of egg-spot number to female choice, the trait is also important in male-male competition, predicting dominance status among males of the same size class. Due to a short lifespan and size assortative mortality [26], rival males are likely to be often of similar size in the wild. This is important also because if the egg-spots are 'badges of status' by honestly indicating a male's position in dominance hierarchy (rela- tive to his size) [25], the observed variation in the number of egg-dummies among males could be more readily understandable. In such a system, cheaters would be punished by their rivals [34-36].

A dominant A. burtoni male usually spreads his anal fin during courtship and male-male interactions, suggesting that the fish are likely to use the signal potential of egg- spots in the context of sexual selection. However, even if the high success of males with a large number of egg-spots was purely countable to correlates of the trait, some disadvantages associated with a high egg-spot

number (or its correlates) should be expected, given the observed high within-population variation in the trait.

These potential disadvantages include the above- mentioned social costs, physiological costs of producing or maintaining spots [23,37] and higher predation pressure on males with many bright spots [38-42]. For example, in small tropical fishes called swordtails (genus Xiphophorus) , females prefer males with a brightly coloured and long extension of their caudal fin (i.e.

'sword'), but these males are also preferably targeted by predatory fish [43,44]. However, we are not aware of any empirical tests on the potential costs of high egg-spot number in A. burtoni or other haplochromines.

Given that individual differences in egg-spot number can be related to passing of time, as well as a male's dom- inance status (which may, in turn, change quickly and be under hormonal control, see [16,17]), the question arises whether these within-population differences are solely under environmental control and strongly affected by phenotypic plasticity. Interestingly, our results suggest that this is not the case, since the differences among males in egg-spot number have a significant heritable component. Specifically, a male with many spots tended to sire male offspring with a high egg-spot number, resulting in a high estimate for narrow sense heritability (h2 = 0.50) of this conspicuous trait. This finding is notable because, obviously, traits can respond to selection only if they have a heritable component. In this respect, egg-spot number seems to be similar to the length of caudal fin extensions in male swordtails, a trait under sexual selection and clear genetic control [45]. More gen- erally, although mating success and fecundity may cause more persistent directional evolution than selection through survival [46], and most traits associated with fitness are expected to have low heritability values [15,47,48], traits involved in sexual selection have recur- rently been reported to have relatively high heritabilities (reviewed in [49]). For example, in collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis), the size of a male's forehead patch had narrow sense heritability of h2

=

0.381 [50]. In a population of guppies (Poecilia reticualata) the number of orange-coloured later spots, a trait relevant to female mate choice, had an estimated heritability as high as h2 = 0.62 [51]. Regarding these comparisons, it is worth noting that our estimate (h2 0.50) might have been somewhat influenced by the fact that, for logistical reasons, the male offspring needed to be photographed at a slightly younger age than their fathers; it is possible that the lower range of trait values in the former than the latter (figure 4) underestimated the strength of regression.

The large within-population variation in the number of egg-spots found in this study is also relevant regarding earlier research suggesting that the number of egg-spots plays a role in species recognition [8]. Specifically, the large range of egg-spot number in A. burtoni, and some other species [14], means that there is likely to be con- siderable overlap in the number of egg-spots among many species, albeit males of some species always have a lower and more consistent egg-spot number [5,7,12].

Our results may therefore call for re-evaluation of egg- spots as a species recognition cue, although we also note that it is still possible that the arrangement or appearance (such as size) of egg-spots is more species-specific

(6)

than their number [5,7,8]. Accordingly, in the current study, we also assessed the average size of each male's egg-spots. When males at the age of 10.5 months were isolated from other individuals, the size of spots slightly increased over time. We found no other consistent patterns concerning the size of the egg-spots in relation to passing of time, body size, dominance status or condition of males. Similarly, heritability of the average size of egg-spots was not significant. These findings are not sur- prising since new, emerging spots are initially small, and we also found that large spots sometimes ended up divid- ing into two smaller spots in older fish. Furthermore, one might expect that the final size of the spots could be under stabilizing selection, for example if they truly functioned as egg mimics [6] or if any sexual selection pressures on egg- spot size are counter-selected by natural selection [52,53].

However, neither hypothesis has been rigorously tested in cichlid fishes. Moreover, females do not necessarily prefer egg-spots that are of the same size as the ova they produce [8,12].

Here, we have provided insights into the proximate and ultimate factors that contribute to the variation in the number of egg-spots on anal fins of male haplochromine cichlid fish. Multiple, potentially interacting, mechanisms seem to be involved: dominant males in good condition are likely to have many egg-spots, the number of which increases over time. Interestingly, the differences in the egg-spot number are highly heritable. Hence, it seems that the primary function of egg-spot number could lie in sexual selection rather than species recognition or fer- tility assurance. Future work will also have to further investigate the developmental proximate mechanisms of this key trait with special evolutionary importance [5,7].

We thank Janine Sieling for logistical help, Birgit Gogol for help with measuring the photographs, Hysing Bolstad and Carla Sgro for their advice on statistical matters, and Frederico Henning, Matthias Sanetra and two anonymous referees for helpful comments and discussions. All animal experiments were approved by the German authorities. The funding was provided by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation fellowship and the Academy of Finland grant to T.K.L. and by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the University of Konstanz to AM.

REFERENCES

Meyer, A 1993 Phylogenetic relationships and evolutionary processes in East African cichlid fishes. Trends Ecol. Eval. 8, 279-284. (doi: 1 0.10 16/0 169-5347(93)90255-N) 2 Stiassny, M. L. J. & Meyer, A 1999 Cichlids of the rift

lakes. Sci. Am. 280, 64-69. (doi:10.1038/scientificamer- ican0299-64)

3 Kocher, T. D. 2004 Adaptive evolution and explosive speciation: the cichlid fish model. Nat. Rev. Genet. 5, 288-298. (doi:10.1038/nrg1316)

4 Seehausen, O. 2006 African cichlid fish: a model system in adaptive radiation research. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 1987- 1998. (doi: 1 0.1098/rspb.2006.3539)

5 Salzburger,

w.,

Mack, T., Verheyen, E. & Meyer, A. 2005 Out of Tanganyika: genesis, explosive speciation, key- innovations and phylogeography of the haplochromine cichlid fishes. BMC Evo!. Bio!. 5, 17. (doi:10.11861 1471-2148-5-17)

6 Wickler, W. 1962 'Egg-dummies' as natural releasers in mouth-breeding cichlids. Nature 194, 1092-1093.

(doi:10.1038/1941092aO)

7 Salzburger, W., Braasch, 1. & Meyer, A 2007 Adaptive sequence evolution in a color gene involved in the for- mation of the characteristic egg-dummies of male haplochromine cichlid fishes. BMC Bioi. 5, 51. (doi:10.

1186/1741-7007-5-51)

8 Goldschmidt, T. & de Visser, J. 1990 On the possible role of egg mimics in speciation. Acta Biotheor. 38, 125-134.

(doi: 1 0.1007 IBF00047549)

9 Hert, E. 1989 The function of egg-spots in an African mouth-brooding cichlid fish. Anim. Behav. 37, 726- 732. (doi: 1 0.10 16/0003-3472(89)90058-4)

10 Tobler, M. 2006 The eggspots of cichlids: evolution through sensory exploitation? Z. Fischkunde 8, 39-46.

11 Capron de Caprona, M.-D. & Fritzsch, B. 1984 Interspecific fertile hybrids of haplochromine cichlidae (teleostei) and their possible importance for specia- tion. Neth. J Zool. 34, 503-538. (doi:10.11631 002829684X00272)

12 Couldridge, V. C. K. 2002 Experimental manipulation of male eggspots demonstrates female preference for one large spot in Pseudotropheus lombardoi. J Fish Bioi. 60, 726-730. (doi:10.11111j.1095-8649.2002.tb01696.x) 13 Ferno, A. 1987 Aggressive behaviour between territorial

cichlids (Astatotilapia burtom) in relation to rank and ter- ritorial stability. Behaviour 103,241-258. (doi:10.11631 156853987XOO 189)

14 Balon, E. K. 1977 Early ontogeny of Labeotropheus Ahl, 1927 (Mbuna, Cichlidae, Lake Malawi), with a discus- sion on advanced protective styles in fish reproduction and development. Environ. Bio!. Fish 2, 147-176.

(doi: 10.1007 IBF000053 7 0)

15 Merila, J. & Sheldon, B. C. 2000 Lifetime reproductive success and heritability in nature. Am. Nat. 155, 301-310. (doi:10.1086/303330)

16 Burmeister, S. S., Jarvis, E. D. & Fernald, R. D. 2005 Rapid behavioral and genomic responses to social oppor- tunity. PLoS Bio!. 3, e363. (doi:10.13711journal.pbio.

0030363)

17 Parikh, V. N., Clement, T. S. & Fernald, R. D. 2006 Androgen level and male social status in the African cichlid, Astatotilapia burtoni. Behav. Brain Res. 166, 291-295. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.011)

18 Grosenick, L., Clement, T. S. & Fernald, R. D. 2007 Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature 445,429-432. (doi:10.1038/nature05511)

19 Hoegg, S., Boore, J. L., Kuehl,

J.

V. & Meyer, A. 2007 Comparative phylogenomic analyses of teleost fish Hox gene clusters: lessons from the cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni. BMC Genom. 8,317. (doi:10.1186/1471-2164- 8-317)

20 Siegel, N., Hoegg, S., Salzburger,

w.,

Braasch, I. &

Meyer, A 2007 Comparative genomics ofParaHox clus- ters of teleost fishes: gene cluster breakup and the retention of gene sets following whole genome dupli- cations. BMC Genom. 8,312. (doi:10.1186/1471-2164- 8-312)

21 Salzburger,

w.,

Renn, S. C. P., Steinke, D., Braasch, I., Hof- mann, H. A. & Meyer, A. 2008 Annotation of expressed sequence tags for the East African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni and evolutionary analyses of cichlid ORFs. BMC Genom. 9,96. (doi:10.1186/1471-2164-9-96)

22 Sanetra, M., Henning, E, Fukamachi, S. & Meyer, A.

2009 A microsatellite-based genetic linkage map of the cichlid fish, Astatotilapia burtoni (Teleostei): a comparison of genomic architectures among rapidly speciating cichlids. Genetics 182, 387 -397. (doi: 10. 1534/genetics.

108.089367)

23 Jennions, M. D., M0ller, A P. & Petrie, M. 2001 Sexually selected traits and adult survival: A meta-analysis.

Q. Rev. Bio!. 76,3-36. (doi:lO.1086/393743)

(7)

24 Johnstone, R A., Rands, S. A. & Evans, M. R. 2009 Sexual selection and condition-dependence. J Evol.

BioI. 22, 2387-2394. (doi:10.11111j.1420-9101.2009.

01822.x)

25 Rohwer, S. 1982 The Evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. Am. Zool. 22, 531-546.

(doi: 10.1 093/icb/22.3.531)

26 Mulimbwa, N., Sarvala, J. & van der Knaap, M. 2010 The effect of intensive fishing on two cichlids (Oreochromis nilo- ticus and Astatotilapia burton!) in Nyangara wetland at the north end of Lake Tanganyika (D. R Congo) with fishery management implications. Aquat. Ecosyst. Health 13, 11-19. (doi:10.1080/14634981003627342)

27 Fernald, R D. & Hirata, N. R 1977 Field study of Hap- lochromis burtoni: habitats and co-habitant. Environ. Bio!.

Fish 2, 299-308. (doi:10.1007/BF00005997)

28 Lynch, M. & Walsh, B. 1998 Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

29 Brooks, R & Kemp, D. J. 2001 Can older males deliver the good genes? Trends Eco!. Evol. 16, 308-3l3. (doi:10.

10 16/S0 169-5347(01)02147-4)

30 Rowe, L. & Houle, D. 1996 The lek paradox and the cap- ture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits.

Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 1415-1421. (doi:10.1098/

rspb.1996.0207)

31 Hert, E. 1991 Female choice based on egg-spots in Pseu- dotropheus aurora Burgess 1976, a rock-dwelling cichlid of Lake Malawi, Africa. J Fish BioI. 38,951-953. (doi:10.

11111j.1095-8649.1991.tb03634.x)

32 Couldridge, V. C. K. & Alexander, G. J. 2001 Does the time spent near a male predict female mate choice in a Malawian cichlid? J Fish Bioi. 59, 667-672. (doi:10.

1 006/jtbi.200 1.1679)

33 Korzana, W. J., Robison, R R, Zhao, S. & Fernald, R D.

2008 Color change as a potential behavioral strategy.

Horm. Behav. 54,463-470. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.

05.006)

34 Candolin, U. 2000 Increased signalling effort when survi- val prospects decrease: male-male competition ensures honesty. Anim. Behav. 60,417-422. (doi:10.1006/anbe.

2000.1481)

35 Candolin, U. 2000 Male-male competition ensures honest signaling of male parental ability in the three- spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aeuleatus). Behav. Eeol.

Soeiobio!. 49,57-61. (doi:10.1007/s002650000267) 36 Parker, T. H. & Ligon, J. D. 2002 Dominant male red

junglefowl (Gallus gallus) test the dominance status of other males. Behav. Ecol. Soeiobio!. 53, 20-24. (doi:10.

1007/s00265-002-0544-5)

37 Hill, G. E. 1999 Is there an immunological cost to caro- tenoid-based ornamental coloration? Am. Nat. 154, 589- 595. (doi: 10.1086/303264)

38 Grether, G. F. 1997 Survival cost of an intrasexually selected ornament in a damselfly. Proe. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 207 -210. (doi:10. 1098/rspb. 1997 .0029)

39 Godin, J.-G.-J. & McDonough, H. E. 2003 Predator pre- ference for brightly colored males in the guppy: a viability

cost for a sexually selected trait. Behav. Eeol. 14, 194- 200. (doi: 10. 1093lbeheco/14.2. 194)

40 Pruden, A. J. & Uetz, G. W. 2004 Assessment of potential predation costs of male decoration and courtship display in wolf spiders using video digitization and playback.

J Insect Behav. 17, 67-80. (doi:10.1023/B:JOIR.

0000025133.54118.1e)

41 Langerhans, R B., Layman, C. A. & DeWitt, T. J. 2005 Male genital size reflects a tradeoff between attracting mates and avoiding predators in two live-bearing fish species. Proc. Natl Aead. Sei. 102, 7618-7623. (doi:10.

1073/pnas.0500935102)

42 Hurtado-Gonzales, J. L. & Baldassarre, J. A C. 2010 Interaction between female mating preferences and pre- dation may explain the maintenance of rare males in the pentamorphic fish Poeeilia parae. J Evol. BioI. 23, 1293-l301. (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01995.x) 43 Rosenthal, G. G., Flores Martinez, T. Y., Garcia de

Leon, F. J. & Ryan, M. J. 2001 Shared preferences by predators and females for male ornaments in swordtails.

Am. Nat. 158, 146-154. (doi:10.1086/321309)

44 Basolo, A L. & Wagner Jr,

w.

E. 2004 Covariation between predation risk, body size and fin elaboration in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri. BioI. J Linn.

Soe. 83, 87-100. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2004.

00369.x)

45 Meyer, A, Salzburger, W. & Schartl, M. 2006 Hybrid origin of a swordtail species (Teleostei: Xiphophorus clemeneiae) driven by sexual selection. Mol. Ecol. 15, 721-730. (doi: 10.11111j.1365-294X.2006.02810.x) 46 Siepielski, A. M., DiBattista, J. D., Evans, J. A &

Carlson, S. M. 2010 Differences in the temporal dynamics of phenotypic selection among fitness com- ponents in the wild. Proc. R. Soc. B 277 (doi:10.1098/

rspb.20 1 0.1973)

47 Blows, M. W. & Hoffmann, A. A. 2005 A reassessment of genetic limits to evolutionary change. Ecology 86, 1371- 1384. (doi:10.1890/04-1209)

48 Carlson, S. M. & Seamons, T. R. 2008 A review of quan- titative genetic components of fitness in salmonids:

implications for adaptation to future change. Evol. Appl.

1, 222-238. (doi: 10.11111j.1752-4571.2008.00025.x) 49 Bakker, T. C. M. 1999 The study of intersexual selection

using quantitative genetics. Behaviour 136, 1237 -1265.

(doi:10.1163/156853999501748)

50 Qvarnstrom, A, Brommer, J. E. & Gustafsson, L. 2006 Testing the genetics underlying the co-evolution of mate choice and ornament in the wild. Nature 441, 84-86.

(doi: 1 0.1 038/nature04564)

51 Karino, K. & Haijima, Y. 2001 Heritability of male sec- ondary sexual traits in feral guppies in Japan. J Etho!.

19,33-37. (doi:10.1007/s101640170015)

52 Endler, J. A 1982 Convergent and divergent effects of natural selection on colour patterns in two fish faunas.

Evolution 36, 178-188. (doi: 1 0.2307/2407979)

53 Andersson, M. 1994 Sexual selection. Priceton, NJ:

Priceton University Press.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

The carpometacarpus is well preserved in the type specimen and closely resembles that of other messelirrisorids, although the processus pisiformis is shifted slightly farther

Phylogeny of the Lake Tanganyika cichlid species flock and its relationship to the Central and East African haplochromine cichlid fish faunas. Speciation via introgressive

Background: Teleost fish have seven paralogous clusters of Hox genes stemming from two complete genome duplications early in vertebrate evolution, and an additional genome

For detailed evolutionary analy- ses, we determined the DNA sequence of this gene locus in 19 East African cichlid species and tested for the signa- ture of adaptive evolution in

The present study aims to analyze a) the response of eggspot numbers to bidirectional selection b) the phenotypic and genetic correlations with body size; c) the strength of

Here we report the collection and annotation of more than 12,000 expressed sequence tags (ESTs) generated from three different cDNA libraries obtained from the East

burloni linkage map was compared to the other two available maps for cichlids using shared markers that showed conservation and synteny among East African cichlid

It remains unclear, whether or not the repeated endemic radiations of Midas cichlids in Nicaraguan crater-lakes are facilitated by phenotypic plasticity in the pharyngeal jaw or if