• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

The effects of autonomy support on salivary alpha-amylase : The role of individual differences

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "The effects of autonomy support on salivary alpha-amylase : The role of individual differences"

Copied!
6
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

The effects of autonomy support on salivary alpha-amylase: The role of individual differences

Vanda Sieber

a,∗

, Julia Schüler

b

, Mirko Wegner

c

aUniversityofZurich,Switzerland

bUniversityofKonstanz,Germany

cUniversityofBern,Switzerland

a b s t r a c t

Theempiricalevidencefortherelationshipbetweenautonomy-supportiveenvironmentsandphysio- logicalstressisinconsistent.Whereassomestudiesreportadecreaseinstressinautonomy-supportive environments,otherstudiesshowanegativeeffectofautonomyonphysiologicalstress. Asprevi- ousresearchhasnotconsideredindividualdifferenceswithinthisrelationship,thepresentresearch aimstoclosethisempiricalgapbyproposingthatanimplicitautonomydisposition,whichisdefined asa dispositionalpreference forself-determination, serves as amoderator. In anexperiment, we testedwhethertheautonomydispositionmoderatestheeffectofdifferentteachingstyles(controlling, autonomy-supportive,andneutral)ontheacutephysiologicalstressresponse(salivaryalpha-amylase) inadolescents(N=69).Thestudyrevealedthatparticipantswithahighimplicitautonomydisposition displayedlowersalivaryalpha-amylaseresponseswhenexposedtoautonomy-supportivevignettescom- paredtowhentheywereexposedtocontrollingorneutralteachingstyles.Theoppositepatternwasfound instudentswithalowimplicitautonomydisposition.Theresultsillustratethatexperimentallyinduced variationsinautonomysupportleadtodifferentphysiologicalstressresponses,dependingonindividual differencesintheimplicitautonomydisposition.

1. Introduction

Several situations in everyday life can lead to psychosocial stress,whichisknowntobereflectedinphysiologicalprocesses andcanconstituteaserioushealthrisk(DickersonandKemeny, 2004).Oneimportantaspectaffectingstressineverydaysituations isthebehaviorofpersonsinhigherhierarchicalpositions,suchas superiorsintheworkcontext(Nieetal.,2015)orteachersatschool (ReeveandTseng,2011).Previousstudieshaveconvincinglyshown thatautonomysupporthasbeneficialeffectsondifferentfacetsof well-beingsuchasvitalityandlifesatisfactionacrossdifferentcon- texts(Adieetal.,2008;Levesqueetal.,2004;Patricketal.,2000).

However,therearealsostudiesshowingthatautonomy,suchasthe possibilitytomakechoices,maydecreasepsychologicalwell-being (GrantandSchwartz,2011;Schwartz,2000;SchwartzandWard, 2004).Therearefewerstudiestestingtherelationshipbetween autonomysupportandphysiologicalstress.Theexistingstudies

Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:sieber@psychologie.uzh.ch(V.Sieber).

onthislink(ReeveandTseng,2011;O’Donnelletal.,2015)have revealedinconsistenciesanditisnotclearwhetherautonomyis associatedwithanincreaseoradecreaseinphysiologicalstress.

Inaschoolcontext,ReeveandTseng(2011)wereabletoidentify effectsofteachers’autonomysupportonstudents’cortisollevels.

Applyingreasoningfromself-determinationtheory(DeciandRyan, 1985,2000),whichstatesthatpeoplebenefitfromautonomysup- portReeveandTseng(2011),foundthatchildrenconfrontedwith acontrollingteacherdisplayedhigherphysiologicalstressduring a learningactivitythanchildrenconfrontedwithanautonomy- supportiveteacher.Theschoolcontextisofparticularimportance forthepresentstudybecauseautonomyhasbeenpreviouslyshown topositivelyaffectareasthatarerelevantforchildren’spsycholog- icalwell-being(RyanandGrolnick,1986),motivation(Reeveetal., 2004),andperformanceinschool(BlackandDeci,2000).

Incontrasttothisevidenceforthepositivestress-reducingeffect ofautonomy-supportiveenvironments,adifferentstudyillustrated thatautonomycanalsoelevatestress.Arecentlypublishedstudyby O’Donnelletal.(2015)foundthatpeopledisplayedahigherphys- iologicalstressresponse,asreflectedintheirheartratevariability, heartrate,andsalivaryalpha-amylase,whengiventheopportunity

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-0-382599 Erschienen in: Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 74 (2016). - S. 173-178

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.003

(2)

tochoosethetimingoftheirbreaksduringawordprocessingtask inasimulatedofficeenvironment.

Evenapplyingthenecessaryamountofcautionwhencompar- ingthesestudies(differentsetting,differentsamples,anddifferent indicatorsforphysiologicalstress),theyrevealacertaininconsis- tencyabouttheeffectofautonomy-supportivesocialenvironments onphysiologicalstressresponses.Consideringthevastliterature ontheimpactofautonomy-supportivesocialenvironmentsinthe schoolcontextondifferentaspectsofwell-beingandmotivation (RyanandGrolnick,1986;Reeveetal.,2004;BlackandDeci,2000), thepresent studyexamines the effect of autonomy-supportive socialenvironmentsin aschoolcontext,more precisein physi- caleducation.Thissettingisconsideredasparticularlyimportant, aspositiveexperiencesinphysicaleducationclassesareseenas animportantaspectcontributingtoamoreactivelife-style and thereforecancontributetoahealthierlife(Haggeretal.,2003).

Moreover,thepresentresearchsuggestsanautonomydisposition asamoderatingvariable.

Theautonomydispositionisdefinedasastableconcerntoexpe- riencepsychologicalfreedomandtheendeavortofeeloneselfto betheactingagentacrossdifferentsituations(DeCharms,1968;

Schüleretal.,2014).Likeotherimplicitdispositions,whichhave beenshowntoaffectphysiologicalstressresponses(Wegneretal., 2014),theimplicitautonomydispositionisassumedtoreflectnon- consciousandaffect-basedpreferencesforcertainincentives(e.g.

feelingautonomous)(Schüleretal.,2014).Intwostudies,Schüler etal.(2014)foundthatautonomysupportwasespeciallybeneficial intermsofmotivationandwell-beingforpeoplewithamoderate tohighimplicitautonomydispositionbutnotforpeoplewithalow implicitautonomydisposition.Thisresultfullyconfirmsa“match- inghypothesis”(e.g.HoferandBusch,2011;Schüleretal.,2013;

Schüleretal.,2010;SheldonandSchüler,2011),accordingtowhich individualsbenefitiftheirpersonality(e.g.,achievementmotive, autonomydisposition)fitstheirsocialenvironment(e.g.,oppor- tunityforexperiencesofcompetenceorautonomy,respectively).

However,theexistingworkonthematchinghypothesishassofar notanalyzedtheeffectsonstress.Thepresentresearchfillsthisgap bytestingthehypothesisthattheimplicitautonomydisposition moderatestheeffectsofanautonomy-supportiveenvironmenton physiological stress.To bemore specific, we analyzedwhether experimentally-inducedautonomysupportinaschoolsportcon- textproduces differenteffects on salivaryalpha-amylase (sAA) levelsinschoolchildrenwithhighandlowautonomydispositions.

Bytesting thismoderatorhypothesis, weaimtoclarifyhow autonomy-supportive environments affect physiological stress reactionsinaschoolcontext.Wedesignedanexperimentinwhich adolescents’physiological short-termreaction toanautonomy- supportiveorautonomy-restrictivesportteacherwasexamined.

In contrast tolong-term reactionsto stress, which are reliably measuredbycortisol–thatbeingtheprincipalhormoneofthe hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) (Ali and Pruessner, 2012; Chrousos, 2009) – short-term and fast stress responses (Skoludaetal.,2015)aremorestronglyrepresentedbytheactivity ofthesympatheticnervoussystem(SNS).TheresponseoftheSNSis veryquick:itactivatespre-ganglionicsympatheticnerves,located in the locus coeruleus. This mechanism triggers the release of norepinephrineandepinephrinefromtheadrenalmedulla,which inturntriggersseveralperipheralreactions,suchasanincreasein salivaryalpha-amylase(sAA),heartrate,andbloodpressure,which canbemeasuredrelativelypromptlyaftertheonsetofthestressor (Ali and Pruessner, 2012; Chrousos, 2009; Naterand Rohleder, 2009).Inrecentresearch,sAAhasbeenshowntoreliablyreflect the short-term reaction to different stressors in experimental situations (Allwood et al., 2011; Nater et al., 2005; Nater and Rohleder,2009;vanStegerenetal.,2006;Skoludaetal.,2015).

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Sixty-ninestudents(34females.1)agedbetween12and16years (M=14.16,SD=1.30)participatedintheexperimentalstudy,which tookplaceonaregularschoolday.Theparticipantswererecruited inhighschoolsintheGerman-speakingpartofSwitzerlandandthe samplewasprimarilyCaucasian(N=63;SwissAfrican:N=3,Swiss Hispanics:N=3).Participationwasonavoluntarybasis.Partici- pantsandtheirparentssignedaninformedconsentformpriorto participation.Thepresentstudywasapprovedbytheethicscom- mitteeoftheFacultyofHumanSciencesattheUniversityofBern, SwitzerlandandisinaccordancewiththeDeclarationofHelsinki.

2.2. Designandprocedure

Theexperimentwasconductedinaregularschoolsetting.Sixty participantsweretested in theafternoonand nine participants weretestedinthemorning.Theparticipantsfirstcompletedthe PictureStoryExerciseinordertoassesstheirimplicitautonomy disposition.Afterwards,participantsgavethefirstsalivasample,so astodeterminetheirbaselinesAA.Inordertoobtainunbiasedsam- ples,theparticipantsrinsedtheirmouthswithwaterandwerenot allowedtotalkwhilethesalivasamplesweretaken.Additionally, theywerenotallowedtoconsumenicotineorcaffeine,eatordrink foratleastonehourbeforethesalivasampling.Afterthesaliva samplehadbeentaken,theyreadthegeneralinstructionsforthe experimentalmanipulationandwererandomlyassignedtoeither anautonomy-supportive(AS,n=27),anautonomy-restrictive(AR, n=22),oracontrolgroup(CG,n=20).Inbothexperimentalgroups (ASandAR),participantswereaskedtoprojecttheirthoughtsinto asetofsevenscenariosinwhichtheyhadtointeractwitheither anautonomy-supportiveoracontrollingsportsteacher(forasim- ilarprocedureseeSchüleretal.,2014).Participantswereaskedto vividlyimaginethescenarios,visualizethem,andfocusontheir thoughtsandemotions.Thescenarioswereformulatedfollowing theitemsfromtheHealthClimateQuestionnaire(Williamsetal., 1996)whichwereadaptedtothesportcontext(e.g.,“Ifeelthat my coachprovidesmewithchoices and options”).Tofacilitate thesubject’simagination,theitemswereembeddedintoatypi- calsceneofaphysicaleducationlessoninschool.IntheASgroup, participantsreadscenarioslike:“Yourphysicaleducationteacher explainsthattherearedifferentwaystocarryoutanexerciseand letsyouchoosethewayyouwanttodoit.”Peopleintheautonomy- restrictivegroupreceivedasimilarscenario,wherethebehavior oftheteacherwaschangedintoautonomy-restrictivebehavior, anexamplebeing:“Yoursportsteachertakesadecisionwithout givingarationaleanddeterminesthewayyouhavetocarryout theexercise.”Meanwhilethecontrolgroupwasinstructedtoread thedescriptionofasporthall.Afterthe10-minvignetteexercise, theparticipantswereaskedtogivea secondsAAsample. After samplecollection,theparticipantsansweredquestionsconcerning medicalissues,medication(includingcontraceptives),substance abuse(e.g.smoking,cannabisconsumption),caffeineconsumption, aswellassleepinghabits.2Afterthecompletionofthequestion- naire,theparticipantsweredebriefedaboutthestudy’saims,and writteninformationwasprovidedincludingcontactdetailsofthe responsiblepersonsofthestudy.

1Noneofthefemaleparticipantswereonbirthcontrol.

2Thesurveycontainedothervariablesthatwerecollectedtoanswerresearch questionsnotrelatedtothepresentone.

(3)

Table1

Summaryofhierarchicalregressionanalysisforvariablespredictingpost-testsAA,with95%biascorrectedandacceleratedconfidenceintervalsreportedinparentheses.

Confidenceintervalsandstandarderrorsbasedon1000bootstrapsamples.

Variable Model1 Model2 Model3

B SEB p B SEB p B SEB p

Timeatsampling 0.367 0.195 0.061 0.374 0.206 0.066 0.433 0.217 0.047

BaselinesAA 2.851 0.240 0.001 2.864 0.264 0.004 2.835 0.261 0.001

ExperimentalGroup

AR-dummy −0.184 0.642 0.770 −0.010 0.651 0.989

CG-dummy −0.255 0.575 0.673 −0.395 0.553 0.493

Autonomydisposition(Aut) 0.045 0.219 0.840 −0.522 0.342 0.138

Experimentalgroup×Aut

AR-dummy×Aut 1.372 0.684 0.046

CG-dummy×Aut 0.956 0.444 0.037

Note:AR:autonomyrestrictiongroup,AS:autonomysupportgroup,CG:controlgroup,AR-dummy(AS:0,AR:1,CG:0),CG-dummy(AS:0,AR:0,CG:1).

Fig.1.Effectofexperimentalmanipulation(AS=autonomysupport,AR=autonomyrestriction,CG=controlgroup)×implicitautonomydispositioninteractiononsquare roottransformedsAA(salivaryalpha-amylase).

2.3. Materials

2.3.1. Implicitautonomydisposition

TheimplicitautonomydispositionwasassessedusingthePic- tureStoryExercise(PSE,SchultheissandPang,2007;Murray,1943).

Inthepresentstudy,participantswereaskedtowriteimaginative storiesaboutfourstandardpictures(stimulusmaterial:womenin alaboratory,ship’scaptain,trapezeartists,andboxer).Thepartici- pantswereinstructedtolookatthepicturefor15sandthenwrite astoryaboutthepictureforfourminutes.Weanalyzedthestories usingscoringcategoriesfromDeCharmsandPlimpton(1992);for asimilarprocedureseeSchüleretal.,2014).Autonomywascoded whenoneofthefollowingcategorieswasmentionedinthesto- ries:(a)self-determinedgoalsetting,(b)instrumentalactivitiesto reachthegoal,(c)realityperception,(d)personalresponsibility, (e)self-confidence,and(f)personal causation.Twoexperienced raterscodedthestories.Theirinter-raterreliability(ICC)was0.94.

Scoringdisagreementswerediscussedandresolvedinadditional sessions.Autonomyscoresrangedfrom0to15(M=6.36,SD=3.79).

Forsubsequentanalyses,thescoreswerecorrectedforwordcount (rangefrom82to333words),astheywerecorrelated(asrecom- mendedbySchultheissandPang,2007).Followingtheguidelines givenbySchultheissandPang(2007),weusedregressionanalysis toresidualizetheimplicitmotivescoresforwordcountandused theresidualscoresintheanalysesreportedbelow.

2.3.2. Salivaryalpha-amylase

Salivaryalpha-amylase(U/ml)wasassessedbylettingpartic- ipantspassivelydrool10mlofsalivaintosamplingtubesusing SaliCaps® (IBLInternational,Hamburg,Germany). SaliCapshave beenpreviouslyusedsuccessfullytoassesssAAindifferentstud- ies(forthisprocedure,seealsoSchoofsandWolf,2009;Sumter etal.,2010).Aftercollection,thesampleswerestoredat−20C untilanalysis(seeRohlederetal.,2004;Strahler,2012).Achemi- luminescenceimmunoassayprocedure(IBL,Hamburg,Germany) wasappliedtoassess sAAlevelsin thesamples.Theintra-and inter-assaycoefficientsfortherawsAAscoreswerebelow3%.As theraw sAAmeasuresweresubstantially skewedfor thebase- line (skew=1.18, SE=0.29) as well as for the post-test scores (skew=0.90,SE=0.29),asquareroottransformationwascarried out(transformedskewbaseline=0.38,SE=0.29,transformedskew post-test=0.28,SE=0.29),inordertoobtainanormaldistribution (forthis procedure, seealso Grangeret al.,2007).Additionally, thescoreswerez-transformedinordertoconductthemoderation analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminaryanalysesanddescriptivestatistics

In preliminary analyses we found that the three exper- imental groups did not differ regarding their baseline sAA

(4)

scores(AS:M=100.07,SD=75.84;AR:M=96.09,SD=53.41;CG:

M=145.91;SD=99.30),F(2,68)=2.695,p=0.08.Correlationalanal- ysesrevealedthatsAAwasnotassociatedwiththeparticipants’age, r=−0.03,p=0.79.Additionally,nosignificantdifferencesinbase- linesAAlevelswerefoundbetweenpeopleassessedinthemorning andintheafternoon,t(9)=−0.83,p=0.43,aswellasforgender,t (66)=1.08,p=0.273.Butthetimeofsamplingwasassociatedwith thedependentmeasureofsAA,r=0.29,p=0.02,andistherefore includedinfurtheranalysesasacontrolvariable.

3.2. Moderationanalysis

Inordertotestthemoderatingeffectoftheimplicitautonomy dispositiononsAAresponsesinthethreeexperimentalgroups,a hierarchicalregressionanalysiswasconductedinthreesteps(see Table1).In thefirstmodel, wecontrolled forthebaseline sAA scores,aswellasforthetimeoftesting(z-transformed).Thesecond modelincludedtheexperimentalmanipulation(AS,AR,CG)and themoderator(implicitautonomydisposition).Inthethirdmodel, theinteractionbetweentheimplicitmotiveandtheexperimen- talconditionswasadded.Inordertotesttheinteractionbetween thethreeexperimentalgroups(AS,AR,CG)andautonomydispo- sition,thetwodummyvariablesAR-dummy(coded1forAR,0for ASand0forCG)andCG-dummy(coded0forAS,0forARund1 forCG)werecreated.Theinteractionterms(Model3)weresig- nificantforAR-dummy×autonomydisposition,b=1.37,seb=0.68, p=0.046,95%BCaCI[0.230,2.962]andforCG-dummy×autonomy disposition,b=0.96,seb=0.44,p=0.037,95%BCaCI[0.100,1.897].4 Thenatureofthisinteractionis depictedinFig.1; peoplewith a high implicit autonomy disposition display the highest sAA scoreswhentheyareexposed toanautonomy-restrictivephys- icaleducationteacher,compared withan autonomy-supportive physicaleducationteacher.In thecontrolgroup, peoplewitha highimplicitautonomydispositiondisplayhighersAAscoresthan thoseintheautonomy-supportivegroup.Incontrast,peoplewith alowimplicit autonomydispositionhadlowersAAlevelsinan autonomy-restrictivesetting,aswellasinthecontrolgroup,but highersAAlevelsintheautonomysupportivegroup.

4. Discussion

Past research examining physiological stress responses to experimentalvariationsin autonomyhasproducedinconsistent results,somefindingpositive andothers negativerelationships.

Thepresentstudyproposesanindividualdifferentiatingvariable, theimplicitautonomydisposition,asanimportantmoderatorof theeffect of autonomysupportonsalivary alpha-amylase.The resultsof the regression analysis illustrate that experimentally inducedvariations in autonomysupport provided by theenvi- ronment(autonomy-supportivevs.autonomy-restrictivebehavior ofaphysicaleducationteachervs.neutraldescriptionofagym) leadtodifferentphysiologicalstressresponsesasreflectedbysAA,

3 Additionallygenderwasnotfoundtobeamoderatoroftheinteractioneffectin themainanalysis,AR-dummy×autonomydisposition×gender,b=1.81,seb=1.43, p=0.79,95%[−2.069,2.710];CG-dummy×autonomydisposition×gender,b=0.32, seb=1.92,p=0.22,95%[−1.142,4.761].

4 Theinspectionoftotalwordcountindicatesthatfourparticipantswrotestories whichwereshorterthan30words,whichrecurrentlyisconsideredasathreshold forcodingmotiveimagery(c.f.WalkerandAtkinson,1958;HoferandBusch,2011).

Runningtheregressionwithoutthoseparticipantsdoesnotchangetheinteraction pattern(AR-dummy×autonomydisposition,b=1.35,seb=0.69,p=0.045,95%BCa CI[0.185,2.886]).However,theanalysisforthecomparisonbetweenthecontrol groupandtheautonomysupportivegroupwasweaker,anddidnotreachsignifi- cance,CG-dummy×autonomydisposition,b=0.78,seb=0.47,p=0.09,95%BCaCI [−0.134,1.703].

dependingonindividualdifferencesintheimplicitautonomydis- position.

Taking a closer look at the interaction pattern (see Fig. 1) showsthatpeoplewithahighimplicitautonomydispositionin theautonomy-restrictivegroupdisplayhigher sAAscores,indi- catinghigherlevelsofstress,comparedwithpeoplewithahigh implicitautonomydispositionintheautonomy-supportivegroup andthoseinthecontrolgroup.Forpeoplewithalowimplicitauton- omydisposition,theinteractionpatternisreversed:peoplewith alowimplicitautonomydispositionintheautonomy-supportive groupdisplaythehighestsAAscorescomparedwiththeothertwo experimentalgroups(controlgroupandautonomyrestriction).

Thisresultisinlinewiththeexistingempiricalevidencefrom thematchinghypothesisproposedbySchüleretal.(2013,2014), which states thatpeoplediffer in howtheyreact toautonomy supportprovidedbytheenvironmentdependingontheirimplicit autonomydisposition.Thisstudycontributestothesefindingsby showing that a fit betweendisposition and environment leads todecreasedsalivaryalpha-amylaselevelswhereas amismatch betweenthesevariables leadstoincreasedlevels.Thus, forthe firsttime,aphysiologicalstressresponsehasbeenshowntobe dependentonthedisposition–environmentinteraction.

Thisfindingisofhightheoreticalrelevance,asitprovidesan insightintohowautonomyaffectsphysiologicalstress.Moreover, itcanbeseenasafirststeptoexplainingwhydifferentstudiescame todivergingconclusionsconcerningtheimpactofautonomyon physiologicalstress(O’Donnelletal.,2015;ReeveandTseng,2011).

Additionally,ourresultshaveahighpracticalrelevance,asthey suggestthattheimplicitautonomydisposition–asamoderatorof thelinkbetweenautonomysupportandphysiologicalstress–is animportantfactortoconsiderwhendesigninghealth-promoting schoolenvironments.

5. Limitationsandfurtherdirections

Whereasthepresentstudyfocusedonashort-termphysiologi- calstressreaction,futureresearchisstronglyencouragedtoassess whetheramismatchbetweentheimplicitautonomydisposition andlong-lastingautonomysupportfromtheenvironmentcanlead tochronicstress,whichcanbemeasuredwithlong-termstress indicators,suchascortisol.Inordertoanalyzelong-termstress reactions,thoroughlycontrolledfieldexperimentsareneeded,in whichforexampleateacher’soracoach’sautonomy-supportive behavior isaltered over a longerinterventionperiod of several weeks.Additionally, future researchmight want toadd further short-termstressindicatorsoftheANS,suchasheartrate,heartrate variability,orskinconductance,inordertoconfirmourfindings withsAA.Moreover,ifapplicable,futureresearchshouldstandard- ize timeof sampling,which would beofadditionalimportance whenanalyzingcortisolknowntobesensitivetodiurnalvariations (DickersonandKemeny,2004).

Oneofthestrengthsofthepresentstudyisthatitexamines sAAresponsesinayoungsampleagedbetween12and16years, addingtothescarceresearchbaseinthisagegroup.However,pre- viousresearchcomparingsAAresponsesindifferentagegroups suggestedthatchildrenandolderadultsshowattenuatedresponse patterns(Strahleretal.,2010).Studiesalsosuggestedthatdiffer- encesinpubertalstatusmightbeassociatedwithsAAresponses tosocialevaluativepsychologicalstressorssuchastheLeidenPub- licSpeakingTask(seevandenBosetal.,2014)orpeerrejection (Stroudetal.,2009).Evenifthetaskinthepresentstudydidnot directlyinvolvesocialevaluation,itwouldbeinterestingforfuture researchtoincludeanobjectivemeasureofpubertalstatusinaddi- tiontotheassessmentofparticipants’age.Thiscouldbedoneby aself-reportquestionnairesuchasthepubertaldevelopmentscale

(5)

(Petersenetal.,1988)orifapplicable,throughaphysicalexami- nationbyatrainedclinicianwhichisconsideredbestpracticeby manyresearchers(seevandenBosetal.,2014).

Althoughthevignettemethodhasseveralstrengths,suchas minimizing theeffects of physical activityonthe physiological measuresAA(seeforexampleNaterandRohleder,2009),future researchmightalsotesttherelationshipexaminedhereinareal- lifesetting,inordertostrengthenthegeneralizabilityoftheresults.

Additionally,futurestudiesemployingvignettescouldimprovethe methodologicalprocedurebyforexampletestingwhetherindivid- ualsdifferintheirabilitiestoimaginethescenarios,andwhether thisinfluencesthereportedresults.Onelimitationofthepresent studyisthatwedidnotmeasurehowmuchautonomysupportthe participantsexperienced.Thiswasduetothefact,thatweuseda questionnairewhichisusuallyusedtomeasureautonomysupport (Williamsetal.,1996)todesignthevignettes.Employingsucha manipulationcheckwasconsideredasredundantinformation.In ordertoavoidadministeringredundantinformationtothepartic- ipants,futureresearchmightforexampleuseavisualanalogscale withanchorwordsrelatedtoautonomysupport(e.g.,“Icouldact completelyself-determined”versus“Ifeltcontrolled”)inorderto provideamanipulationcheck.

Futureresearchmayalsofocusondifferentsettings,suchas workenvironments, health settings,and other educationalset- tings (universities). More research is also needed to examine the differential effect of established leadership styles, such as transformational leadership,which is known to foster thesat- isfaction of basic psychological needs and support a person’s self-determination(Kovjanicetal.,2012).

Additionally, future research might focus on mechanisms underlying the negative effect of autonomy supportive envi- ronments forpeoplewitha low implicit autonomydisposition.

Followingtherationaleofpreviousresearch,studiescouldassess whether the heightened stress response in people with a low implicit disposition is due to more difficulties in goal striving in domains that target thespecific disposition they are low in (seeSchultheissandKöllner,2014).Autonomy supportiveenvi- ronments therefore might act as a stressor for people with a low implicit autonomydisposition, and might lead toeffortful goal-pursuitandlowmotive-drivenhedonicresponse.Moreover, autonomysupportiveenvironmentscouldalsoactasdisincentive (Schultheiss,2008)forotherimplicitmotives(suchasaffiliationor achievement)forpeoplewithalowautonomydisposition.Asacon- sequence,thedisincentivecouldactasstressortotheirdominant implicitmotive(SchultheissandKöllner,2014).

Conflictsofinterest

Theauthorshavenoconflictsofinterestanddeclarenonfinan- cialinterests.Allauthorsdiscloseanyactualorpotentialconflict ofinterestincludinganyfinancial,personalorotherrelationships withotherpeopleororganizationsthatcouldinappropriatelyinflu- ence,orbeperceivedtoinfluence,theirwork.

Contributors

Allauthorswereinvolvedintheconceptionanddesignofthe study.Theacquisitionandanalysisofthedatawasmainlycon- ductedbyVS.VSwrotethefirstdraftofthemanuscript. JSand MWmadesubstantialcontributionstothefirstdraft,byrevising itcritically.Allauthorscontributedandhaveapprovedthefinal manuscript.

Roleofthefundingsource

ThisworkwassupportedbyfundsfromtheFederalOfficeof Sports in Switzerland (Grant NR 37-203). Saliva sampling was fundedbyresearchfundingoftheUniversityofBern,Switzerland (31-375).Bothfundingsourceshad nofurtherrole inthestudy design;collection,analysis,andinterpretationofdata;inwriting thereport;orinthedecisiontosubmitthepaperforpublication.

Acknowledgements

VandaSieberis supportedbytheFederal Officefor Sportin Switzerland(BundesamtfürSport).Additionallywewouldliketo thankManuelaEngelandSamanthaGerberfortheirsupportindata collection.

References

Adie,J.,Duda,J.L.,Ntoumanis,N.,2008.Autonomysupport,basicneedsatisfaction andtheoptimalfunctioningofadultmaleandfemalesportparticipants:atest ofbasicneedstheory.Motiv.Emot.32,189–199,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/

s11031-008-9095-z.

Ali,N.,Pruessner,J.C.,2012.Thesalivaryalphaamylaseovercortisolratioasa markertoassessdysregulationsofthestresssystems.Physiol.Behav.106(1), 65–72,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.10.003.

Allwood,M.A.,Handwerger,K.,Kivlighan,K.T.,Granger,D.A.,Stroud,L.R.,2011.

Directandmoderatinglinksofsalivaryalpha-amylaseandcortisol

stress-reactivitytoyouthbehavioralandemotionaladjustment.Biol.Psychol.

88(1),57–64,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.06.008.

Black,A.E.,Deci,E.L.,2000.Theeffectsofinstructors’autonomysupportand students’autonomousmotivationonlearningorganicchemistry:a self-determinationtheoryperspective.Sci.Educ.84(6),740–756.

Chrousos,G.P.,2009.Stressanddisordersofthestresssystem.Nat.Rev.

Endocrinol.5(7),374–381.

DeCharms,R.,Plimpton,F.,1992.Theoriginscoringsystem.In:Smith,C.R.(Ed.), MotivationandPersonality:HandbookofThematicContentAnalysis.

CambridgeUniversityPress,Cambridge,pp.334–375.

DeCharms,R.,1968.PersonalCausation.AcademicPress,NewYork.

Deci,E.L.,Ryan,R.M.,1985.IntrinsicMotivationandSelf-DeterminationinHuman Behaviour.Plenum,NewYork.

Deci,E.L.,Ryan,R.M.,2000.Thewhatandwhyofgoalpursuits:humanneedsand theself-determinationofbehavior.Psychol.Inq.11(4),227–268.

Dickerson,S.S.,Kemeny,M.E.,2004.Acutestressorsandcortisolresponses:a theoreticalintegrationandsynthesisoflaboratoryresearch.Psychol.Bull.130 (3),355–391,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355.

Granger,D.A.,Kivlighan,K.T.,El-Sheikh,M.,Gordis,E.B.,Stroud,L.R.,2007.

Salivary-amylaseinbiobehavioralresearch:recentdevelopmentsand applications.Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci.1098(1),122–144,http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/

annals.1384.008.

Grant,A.M.,Schwartz,B.,2011.Toomuchofagoodthing:thechallengeand opportunityoftheinvertedU.perspect.Psychol.Sci.6(1),61–76,http://dx.

doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393523.

Hagger,M.S.,Chatzisarantis,NikosL.D.,Culverhouse,T.,Biddle,StuartJ.H.,2003.

Theprocessesbywhichperceivedautonomysupportinphysicaleducation promotesleisure-timephysicalactivityintentionsandbehavior:a

trans-contextualmodel.J.Educ.Psychol.95(4),784–795,http://dx.doi.org/10.

1037/0022-0663.95.4.784.

Hofer,J.,Busch,H.,2011.Satisfyingone’sneedsforcompetenceandrelatedness:

consequentdomain-specificwell-beingdependsonstrengthofimplicit motives.Pers.Soc.Psychol.B37(9),1147–1158,http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/

0146167211408329.

Kovjanic,S.,Schuh,S.C.,Jonas,K.,vanQuaquebeke,N.,vanDick,R.,2012.Howdo transformationalleadersfosterpositiveemployeeoutcomes?A

self-determination-basedanalysisofemployees’needsasmediatinglinks.J.

Organiz.Behav.33(8),1031–1052,http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.1771.

Levesque,C.,Zuehlke,A.N.,Stanek,L.R.,Ryan,R.M.,2004.Autonomyand competenceinGermanandAmericanUniversitystudents:acomparative studybasedonself-determinationtheory.J.Educ.Psychol.96(1),68–84, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.68.

Murray,H.A.,1943.ThematicApperceptionTest.HarvardUniversityPress, Cambridge,MA.

Nater,U.M.,Rohleder,N.,2009.Salivaryalpha-amylaseasanon-invasive biomarkerforthesympatheticnervoussystem:currentstateofresearch.

Psychoneuroendocrinology34(4),486–496,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2009.01.014.

Nater,U.M.,Rohleder,N.,Gaab,J.,Berger,S.,Jud,A.,Kirschbaum,C.,Ehlert,U., 2005.Humansalivaryalpha-amylasereactivityinapsychosocialstress paradigm.Int.J.Psychophysiol.55(3),333–342,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

ijpsycho.2004.09.009.

(6)

Nie,Y.,Chua,B.L.,Yeung,A.S.,Ryan,R.M.,Chan,W.Y.,2015.Theimportanceof autonomysupportandthemediatingroleofworkmotivationforwell-being:

testingself-determinationtheoryinaChineseworkorganisation.Int.J.

Psychol.50(4),245–255,http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12110.

O’Donnell,E.,Landolt,K.,Hazi,A.,Dragano,N.,Wright,B.J.,2015.Anexperimental studyofthejobdemand-controlmodelwithmeasuresofheartratevariability andsalivaryalpha-amylase:evidenceofincreasedstressresponsesto increasedbreakautonomy.Psychoneuroendocrinology51,24–34,http://dx.

doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.017.

Patrick,B.C.,Hisley,J.,Kempler,T.,2000.What’severybodysoexcitedabout?:the effectsofteacherenthusiasmonstudentintrinsicmotivationandvitality.J.

Exp.Educ.68(3),217–236.

Petersen,A.C.,Crockett,L.,Richards,M.,Boxer,A.,1988.Aself-reportmeasureof pubertalstatus:reliability,validity,andinitialnorms.J.YouthAdolesc.17(2), 117–133,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962.

Reeve,J.,Tseng,C.-M.,2011.Cortisolreactivitytoateacher’smotivatingstyle:the biologyofbeingcontrolledversussupportingautonomy.Motiv.Emot.35(1), 63–74,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9204-2.

Reeve,J.,Jang,H.,Carrell,D.,Jeon,S.,Barch,J.,2004.Enhancingstudents’

engagementbyincreasingteachers’autonomysupport.Motiv.Emot.28(2), 147–169,http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000032312.95499.6f.

Rohleder,N.,Nater,U.M.,Wolf,J.M.,Ehlert,U.,Kirschbaum,C.,2004.Psychosocial stress-inducedactivationofsalivaryalpha-amylase:anindicatorof

sympatheticactivity?Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci.1032(1),258–263,http://dx.doi.org/

10.1196/annals.1314.033.

Ryan,R.M.,Grolnick,W.S.,1986.Originsandpawnsintheclassroom:self-report andprojectiveassessmentsofindividualdifferencesinchildren’sperceptions.

J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.50(3),550–558.

Schüler,J.,Sheldon,K.M.,Fröhlich,S.M.,2010.Implicitneedforachievement moderatestherelationshipbetweencompetenceneedsatisfactionand subsequentmotivation.J.Res.Pers.44(1),1–12,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

jrp.2009.09.002.

Schüler,J.,Brandstätter,V.,Sheldon,K.M.,2013.Doimplicitmotivesandbasic psychologicalneedsinteracttopredictwell-beingandflow?Testinga universalhypothesisandamatchinghypothesis.Motiv.Emot.37(3),480–495, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9317-2.

Schüler,J.,Sheldon,K.M.,Prentice,M.,Halusic,M.,2014.Dosomepeopleneed autonomymorethanothers?Implicitdispositionstowardautonomy moderatetheeffectsoffeltautonomyonwell-being.J.Pers.,http://dx.doi.org/

10.1111/jopy.12133.

Schoofs,D.,Wolf,O.T.,2009.Stressandmemoryretrievalinwomen:nostrong impairingeffectduringthelutealphase.Behav.Neurosci.123(3),547–554, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015625.

Schultheiss,O.C.,Köllner,M.G.,2014.Implicitmotives,affect,andthedevelopment ofcompetencies:avirtuous-circlemodelofmotive-drivenlearning.In:

Pekrun,R.,Linnenbrink-Garcia,L.(Eds.),InternationalHandbookofEmotions inEducation.Taylor&Francis/Routledge,NewYork,pp.73–95.

Schultheiss,O.C.,Pang,J.S.,2007.Measuringimplicitmotives.In:Robins,R.W., Fraley,R.C.,Krueger,R.(Eds.),HandbookofResearchMethodsinPersonality Psychology.Guilford,NewYork,pp.322–344.

Schultheiss,O.C.,2008.Implicitmotives.In:John,O.P.,Robins,R.W.,Pervin,L.A.

(Eds.),HandbookofPersonality:TheoryandResearch.Guilford,NewYork,pp.

603–633.

Schwartz,B.,Ward,A.,2004.Doingbetterbutfeelingworse:theparadoxofchoice.

In:Linley,P.A.,Joseph,S.(Eds.),PositivePsychologyinPractice.Wiley, Hoboken,N.J,pp.86–104.

Schwartz,B.,2000.Self-determination:thetyrannyoffreedom.Am.Psychol.55 (1),79–88,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.79.

Sheldon,K.M.,Schüler,J.,2011.Wanting,having,andneeding:integratingmotive dispositiontheoryandself-determinationtheory.J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.101(5), 1106–1123,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024952.

Skoluda,N.,Strahler,J.,Schlotz,W.,Niederberger,L.,Marques,S.,Fischer,S., Thoma,M.V.,Spoerri,C.,Ehlert,U.,Nater,U.M.,2015.Intra-individual psychologicalandphysiologicalresponsestoacutelaboratorystressorsof differentintensity.Psychoneuroendocrinology51,227–236,http://dx.doi.org/

10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.002.

Strahler,J.,Mueller,A.,Rosenloecher,F.,Kirschbaum,C.,Rohleder,N.,2010.

Salivaryalpha-amylasestressreactivityacrossdifferentagegroups.

Psychophysiology47(3),587–595,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.

2009.00957.x.

Strahler,J.,2012.Salivaryalpha-amylase.In:Ehrlenspiel,F.,Strahler,K.(Eds.), PsychoneuroendocrinologyofSportandExercise.Routledge,NewYork,pp.

112–138.

Stroud,L.R.,Foster,E.,Papandonatos,G.D.,Handwerger,K.,Granger,D.A., Kivlighan,K.T.,Niaura,R.,2009.Stressresponseandtheadolescenttransition:

performanceversuspeerrejectionstressors.Dev.Psychopathol.21(1),47–68, http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000042.

Sumter,S.R.,Bokhorst,C.L.,Miers,A.C.,vanPelt,J.,Westenberg,P.M.,2010.Ageand pubertydifferencesinstressresponsesduringapublicspeakingtask:do adolescentsgrowmoresensitivetosocialevaluation?

Psychoneuroendocrinology35(10),1510–1516,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2010.05.004.

vanStegeren,A.,Rohleder,N.,Everaerd,W.,Wolf,O.T.,2006.Salivaryalpha amylaseasmarkerforadrenergicactivityduringstress:effectofbetablockade.

Psychoneuroendocrinology31(1),137–141,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2005.05.012.

vandenBos,E.,Rooij,M.,deMiers,A.C.,Bokhorst,C.L.,Westenberg,P.M.,2014.

Adolescents’increasingstressresponsetosocialevaluation:pubertaleffects oncortisolandalpha-amylaseduringpublicspeaking.ChildDev.85(1), 220–236,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12118.

Walker,E.L.,Atkinson,J.W.,1958.Theexpressionoffearrelatedmotivationin thematicapperceptionasafunctionofproximitytoanatomicexplosion.In:

Atkinson,J.W.(Ed.),MotivesinFantasy,Action,andSociety.Princeton,Van Nostrand,pp.143–159.

Wegner,M.,Schüler,J.,Budde,H.,2014.Theimplicitaffiliationmotivemoderates cortisolresponsestoacutepsychosocialstressinhighschoolstudents.

Psychoneuroendocrinology48,162–168,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2014.06.013.

Williams,G.C.,Grow,V.,Freedman,Z.R.,Ryan,R.M.,Deci,E.L.,1996.Motivational predictorsofweightlossandweight-lossmaintenance.J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.70 (1),115–126.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

For this purpose, the methods of Value Stream Design and fact sheets are used to describe the characteristics of production systems objects (resources and parts)..

First, while very little is known about the impacts of school autonomy at the school level, the present study sheds light on how school autonomy makes changes to the

Our assumption of agents heterogenous in their personal Autonomy level furthers this argument: workers should be granted similar high (low) Autonomy on both the project and task

In Absence , we assume that Sarah’s non-existence, e, is the sole truthmaker of ¯ Sarah does not exist. The normative proposition There is no obligation of Sarah to donate is made

This line of reasoning is further supported by the fact that global rela- tionship satisfaction showed a clear pattern of moderating the mean-level bias for every item: The

Assessment of Identity Development in Adolescence AIDA Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders SKID-II Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children HAWIK

It reflects that real world scenarios provide a rich and multitude of interaction possibilities between participating agents which offers a large number of degrees of freedom

His ardent defense of the freedom of research must thus be seen in the context of its time, as a strong reaction to the perceived endangerment of freedom