• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Effect of Morning and Afternoon Naps on Mood After Sleep Deprivation in Depressives

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Effect of Morning and Afternoon Naps on Mood After Sleep Deprivation in Depressives "

Copied!
3
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

C. Sleep and Depression

Sleep '88 • Edited by J. H o m e • pp 2 2 7 - 2 2 9 Gustav Fischer Verlag • Stuttgart - N e w York -1989

Effect of Morning and Afternoon Naps on Mood After Sleep Deprivation in Depressives

M . Wiegand (Munich), D. Riemann (Mannheim), J. Zulley, Ch. Lauer, W. Schreiber, S. Elsenga (Munich), and M . Berger (Mannheim)

According to some anecdotal reports, a nap during the day following successful total sleep deprivation (TSD) can provoke a mood setback in depressive patients (1, 2, 3). In a previous study (4), we systematically examined the effect of daytime naps at 1300 h subsequent to T S D in drug-free depressive patients. About 5 0 % of the naps caused a relapse, sometimes even a dramatic worsening. In a separate analysis of hourly mood ratings before and after the naps, we additionally found a delayed mood setback even in those T S D responders who had not worsened immediately after the nap (5). Our data did not allow definite conclusions to be made on the relationship between nap sleep characteristics and mood changes, but pointed to the roles of longer nap sleep duration and the occurrence of R E M sleep as crucial factors involved in mood worsenings.

In a study on patients treated with antidepressants, Giedke (6) observed mood changes in both directions after naps beginning between 1330 h and 1430 h. Gillin et al. (7) studied the effects of brief (10 minutes) morning vs. afternoon naps (0830 h vs. 1500 h) and did not observe mood worsenings. O n the contrary, mood tended to improve after morning naps in responders to T S D . The aim of the present study was to validate our previous findings concerning the mood worsening properties of naps and to study the influence of nap timing.

Methods

26 inpatients (8 male, 18 female) with a major depressive disorder (DSM-III) participated in the study (mean age 49.1 ± 12.3 years, mean Hamilton Depression Scale - 21 item version score: 27.2 ± 5.4). After a washout period of at least seven days, and an adaptation night in the sleep laboratory, a baseline sleep E E G was recorded. During the following night, the patients were totally sleep deprived. O n the subsequent day, they took a daytime nap at 0900 h or 1500 h (randomly assigned) with sleep E E G recording, and terminated by spontaneous awa- kening. Depressive symptomatology was assessed by means of the Hamilton Depression Scale - 6 item version ( H A M D - 6 ) at 0800 h and 1500 h on the days before and after T S D . After T S D ,

(2)

228 • Free Communications and Posters

additional ratings were obtained at 1000 h (or 30 min after morning nap) and 1700 h (or 30 min following afternoon nap). The response to T S D was defined as a reduction of at least 3 0 % in the score of the morning H A M D - 6 ratings.

Results and Discussion

O f the 26patients, 17 (= 65,4%) responded to total sleep deprivation. There were no differences between responders and nonresponders with respect to age, sex, baseline psychopathology and baseline sleep variables, except for a tendency towards shorter R E M latencies in responders (36.5 ± 26.6 min as compared with 74.6 ± 59.3 min in nonresponders, p < 0.10, Mann-Whitney U-test, two-tailed). The morning nap group comprised of 8 respon- ders and 5 nonresponders. One patient in each group did not fall asleep, and one responder did not wake up spontaneously within three hours and had to be awakened (according to the design). These patients were excluded from the present analysis. The afternoon nap group comprised of 9 responders and 4 nonresponders, all of whom woke up spontaneously.

Regarding nap sleep E E G variables, there were no significant differences between T S D responders vs. nonresponders and morning vs. afternoon naps. Total sleep time, however, tended to be longer in afternoon naps. Only a small number of patients exhibited R E M sleep during the nap.

worse

better

14

12

10

8

A H a m d - 6 g (pre-nap vs.

post-nap) 4

2 -

0 -

-2

-4 J

morning nap

afternoon nap

Fig. 1: M o o d changes during morning vs. afternoon naps in responders to T S D .

(3)

C. Sleep and Depression • 229 During the day after T S D , the nonresponders' mood ratings exhibited only minor variations.

In T S D responders, the mean scores reflected a clear relapse into depression after a morning nap. In contrast, an afternoon nap did not lead to a significant change in the mean mood score.

In morning naps, a worsening of mood appeared to be positively correlated with total sleep time. O n the other hand, even long afternoon naps led only to minor mood changes. Under both conditions, there was no relationship between worsenings and the amount of SWS. R E M sleep only occurred in three naps, without any consistent effect on mood.

Following successful sleep deprivation, morning naps lead more frequently to a relapse into depression than afternoon naps. The degree of worsening after a morning nap appears to be correlated with the duration of nap sleep. In the afternoon, even long naps do not lead to distinct worsenings. The presence of R E M sleep does not seem to be the crucial factor involved in mood worsenings. In our previous study (4), the data had not allowed this conclusion, because the effects of R E M sleep and nap sleep duration could not be separated.

In the light of the S-deficiency hypothesis of depression (8), our findings can be explained by a higher level of process S in the afternoon, leading to a lower risk of relapse into depression due to a longer period of prior wakefulness. However, our data show no clear relationship between mood changes and the amount of SWS during the nap. Alternatively, circadian factors may account for the varying risk of relapse.

References

1. Pflug,B.,T6lle, R., Nervenarzt 4 2 , 1 1 7 - 1 2 4 , 1 9 7 1 .

2. Knowles, J.B., Southmayd, S.E., Delva, N . , MacLean, A . W . , Cairns, J., Letemendia, F.J., Br. J.

Psychiatry 135, 4 0 3 - 4 1 0 , 1 9 7 9 .

3. Roy-Byrne, P.P., Uhde, T. W . , Post, R . M . in: Neurobiology of M o o d Disorders: Post, R . M . Ballenger, J . C (eds) 8 1 7 - 8 3 5 , Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1984.

4. Wiegand, M . , Berger, M . , Zulley, J., Lauer, C , von Zerssen, D . , Biol. Psychiatry 22, 3 8 6 - 3 8 9 , 1987a.

5. Wiegand, M . , Berger, M . , Zulley, J., Lauer, C , Riemann, D . , von Zerssen, D . , Sleep Res. 16,542,1987b.

6. Giedke, H . in: Sleep '86; Koella, W . P., Obal, F., Schulz, H . , Visser, P. (eds) 451 - 4 5 3 , Fischer, Stuttgart, 1988.

7. Gillin, J. C , Risch, S. C , Janowsky, D . , Kripke, D . , Sleep Res. 16, 273, 1987.

8. Borbely, A . , Pharmacopsychiatry 20, 2 3 - 2 9 , 1 9 8 7 .

M . Wiegand, Max-Planck-Institute of Psychiatry, Kraepelinstr. 10, 8000 Miinchen 40 (FRG)

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

A Norwegian study revealed an increased prevalence of obstructive sleep apnea among a group of 51 persons with solvent exposure in comparison with 18 unexposed referents (18)..

(Un)doing Nationalism and Resistance, with the support of the Alpen-Adria-Universität Klagenfurt and under the patronage of the Alps-Adriatic-Rectors’ Conference (AARC), to

showed in a relatively small group of OSA patients (n =7) a significant drop in the Profile of Mood States (POMS) total mood disturbance after 2 months of CPAP treatment compared with

ZĞŐĂƌĚŝŶŐŵĞĐŚĂŶŝƐŵƐƵŶĚĞƌůLJŝŶŐƐůĞĞƉͲǁĂŬĞƌĞŐƵůĂƚŝŽŶ͕ǁĞƌĞĂƉƉƌĂŝƐĞƉƌĞǀŝŽƵƐƌĞƉŽƌƚƐƉƌŽƉŽƐŝŶŐ Ă ŚŝŐŚĞƌ ďƵŝůĚͲƵƉ ŽĨ ƐůĞĞƉ ƉƌĞƐƐƵƌĞ ŝŶ WZϯ ϱͬϱ ĐĂƌƌŝĞƌƐ ĐŽŵƉĂƌĞĚ ƚŽ

At the emergency department of the UHB, Switzerland, 180 male (80 %) and female (20 %) work injury patients with a mean age of 35.5 years were recruited (for detailed findings, see

The present study is intended to fill a research gap con- cerning sleep of couples in the transition to parenthood, since only very few studies have also

Secondly, (2) psychological well-being is associated with sleep quality in both sexes, since previous studies re- ported overall mental health, depression and anxiety to be

Although a few studies indicate that effects on athletic performance might not be present, sleep, mood and some physiological measures are affected due to shift in time