• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

In a cultural context where the model of interdependence prevails, emo-tion regulaemo-tion centers on socially engaging emoemo-tions, on the well-being of other persons, and on maintaining harmony in the group. Attempts to maintain security are largely dependent on acceptance by the group.

In contrast, in a cultural context where the model of independence pre-vails, emotion regulation centers on socially disengaging emotions, on self-esteem, and on individuals' well-being. The development of emotion regulation in non-Western cultures is related to empathy, interpersonal accommodation, and norm orientation. The development of emotion regulation in Western cultures is related to autonomy and self-expression (see Table 4.1).

109

Table 4.1 Cultural differences in emotion regulation and socialization Western Countries

11

The socialization and development of emotion regulation differs in these cultural contexts. In Western cultures, the most important goal for the development of emotion regulation usually is to promote one's autonomy and affirm positive views of the self. The most important goal for emotion regulation in many non-Western cultures is to adapt to social expectations and obligations by accommodating the individual self and protecting the collective self (see Table 4.1). Feeling with the other, under-standing the emotions of the other, and taking into account social rules, social roles, and duties is the primary pathway to the development of emotion regulation in cultures favoring the model of interdependence.

Throughout this chapter we have emphasized ways in which members of Western or non-Western communities regulate emotion. In so doing, we have glossed over important differences between Western communi-ties and between non-Western communicommuni-ties. Equally or more important, we have neglected differences within cultures.

Cultures are not homogenous systems (see critical review by Oyser-man, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002), and descriptions of values on the cultural level should be differentiated from belief systems on the sub-group and individual level (e.g., cohort effects and differences related to socio-economic status have to be taken into account; Trommsdorff, Mayer, & Albert, 2004). Therefore, it makes sense to assess emotion reg-ulation of individuals within as well as between cultures. An interesting study would be to test whether different wa ys of regulating emotion occur during goal-oriented behavior, depending on the respective goal prefer-ence. Cultural differences in the priority given to beliefs in independence and interdependence can be seen as a cultural frame which influences social customs and formal institutions and thereby permeates socializa-tion and the development of emosocializa-tions (see Table 4.1 for a summary of the cultural differences).

We end this chapter by considering two findings that have interesting implications for one another. First, suppressionJlack of expression of neg-ative emotion is more common and more valued in non-Western cultures.

Second, emotional distress is more common in those non-Western cultures (see Trommsdorff et aI., 2007, for children). If, as we claim, people in non-Western cultures are more effective at suppressing negative emotions, why do they experience more distress? There is evidence that this distress is situation-specific. It is most likely to occur in unfamiliar situations (i.e., in novel circumstances in which people cannot rely on previous experi-ence and social norms), and in situations where people are likely to "let their guard down" (i.e., in informal situations and/or in the presence of intimate others). Even though non-Westerners are more effective at sup-pressing (or not exsup-pressing) emotions, they apparently pay a price for

ex-111 ercising this strategy. Yet, the price paid is limited to particular situations, suggesting that the adverse effects of suppression may be less robust among non-Western individuals.

There is reason to believe that the price paid for suppression is sup-pression rebound-the tendency of suppressed thoughts and feelings to reassert themselves, due to effortless monitoring processes. While the function of monitoring is to enable suppression (maintaining vigilance for the to-be-avoided thoughts) the inadvertent effect is to increase awareness of the very thoughts one wishes to ignore. Wenzlaff and Wegner's (2000) work on suppression provides ample evidence of this rebound dynamic, at least in Western communities. The findings reviewed above suggest that these dynamics may be universal, or at least that they also occur in several non-Western communities. The finding that, in non-Western com-munities, these dynamics are manifest only in particular situations points to the likely interplay of universal dynamics (suppression rebound) and culturally influenced processes (the limited situations in which rebound manifests itself and, thus, the decreased likelihood of its occurrence). Our focus throughout this chapter has been the prevalence of that interplay.

REFERENCES

Aaker,

J.

L., & Lee, A. Y. (2006). Understanding regulatory fit. Journal of Mar-keting Research, 43,15-19.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. c., Waters, & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of at-tachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Azuma, H. (1986). Why study child development in Japan? In H. W. Stevenson, H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan (pp. Kew York: Freeman.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

Barber, B. K. (Ed.) (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children and adolescents. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Baumeister, R. E, & Heatherton, T. E (1996). Self-regulation failure: An over-view. Psychological Inquiry, 7,1-15.

Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psy-chology, 4,1-103.

Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child de-velopment today and tomorrow (pp. 349-378). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Bowlby,

J.

(1973). Attachment and loss. Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.

Kew York: Hogarth Press.

Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394-402.

C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1990). Principles of self-regulation: Action and emotion. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation

[11

and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vo!. 2, pp. 3-52). New York:

Guilford.

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cas pi, A., Roberts, B.

w.,

& Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Sta-bility and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453-484.

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style:

Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65,1111-1119.

Chao, R. K. (2000). Cultural explanations for the role of parenting in the school success of Asian-American children. In R. Taylor & M. Wang (Eds.), Resilience across contexts: Family, work, culture, and community (pp. 333-363). Mahwah, NI: Erlbaum.

Cheah, C. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (2003). European American and mainland Chi-nese mothers' socialization beliefs regarding preschoolers' social skills. Parent-ing: Science & Practice, 3,1-21.

Chen, H. (2001). Parents' attitudes and expectations regarding science education:

Comparisons among American, Chinese-American, and Chinese families. Ado-lescence, 36,305-313.

Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Child Development, 76, 182-195.

Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L.

(1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Cana-dian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677-686.

Chiu,

c.,

Dweck, C. S., Tong,

J.

Y., & Fu,

J.

H. (1997). Implicit theories and con-ceptions of morality. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 73,923-940.

Choi, 1., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories and recognition of contradiction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 890-905.

Choi, S. H. (1992). Communicative socialization processes: Korea and Canada.

In S. Iwawaki, Y. Kashima, & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 103-121). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Clancy, P. M. (1986). The acquisition of communicative style in Japanese.

In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 213-250). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cole, M. (1996). Culture in mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Cole, P. M., Martin, S. & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scien-tific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 317-333.

Cole, P. M., & Tamang, B. L. (1998). Nepali children's ideas about emotional dis-plays in hypothetical challenges. Developmental Psychology, 34, 640-646.

Cole, P. M., Tamang, B. L., & Shrestha, S. (2006). Cultural variations in the sociali-zation of young children's anger and shame. Child Development, 77, 1237-1251.

Creighton, M. R. (1990). Revisiting shame and guilt cultures: A forty-year pil-grimage. Ethos, 18, 279-307.

113 Deci, E. L, & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits:

Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.

DeVos, G. A. (1973). Socialisation for achievement: Essays on the cultural psy-chology of the Japanese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Doi, T. (1973). The anatomy of dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha.

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and de-velopment. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Eisenberg, N. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: An emerging construct.

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 236-259.

Eisenberg, N., Liew, J., & Pidada, S. U. (2004). The longitudinal relations of regulation and emotionality to quality of Indonesian children's socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 790-804.

Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L (2004). Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75,334-339.

Elliot, A. J., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y., & Sheldon, K. M. (2001). A cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach) personal goals. Psychological Sci-ence, 12,505-645.

Emde, R. N., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1985). Assessing emotional availability in early development. In W. K. Frankenburg, R. N. Emde, & J. W. Sullivan (Eds.), Early identification of children at risk: An international perspective (pp. 79-101). New York: Plenum.

Fogel, A. (1993). Development through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Fogel, A., Stevenson, M. B., & Messinger, D. (1992). A comparison of the parent-child relationship in Japan and the United States. In J. L Roopnarine &

D. Bruce (Eds.), Annual advances in applied developmental psychology: Vol. 5.

Parent-child socialization in diverse cultures (pp. 25-51). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Fredrickson, B. L, & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dy-namics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678-686.

Friedlmeier, W., & Trommsdorff, G. (1999). Emotion regulation in early child-hood: A cross-cultural comparison between German and Japanese toddlers.

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30,684-711.

Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialization of shame among young Chinese children. Ethos, 27,180-209.

Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2001). Cross-cultural response to failure: Consider-ing outcome attributions with different goals. In F. Salili, C. Chiu, & Y.-Y. Hong (Eds.), Student motivation: The culture and context of learning (pp. 203-219).

New York: Kluwer.

Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541-553.

Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Di-vergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,224-237.

Gross, J. J. (1998b). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299.

[1

Gross,

J. J.

(1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, future. Cognition & Emo-tion, 13, 551-573.

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social con-sequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281-291.

Gross,

J.

J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regula-tion processes: Implicaregula-tions for affect, relaregula-tionships, and well-being. journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,348-362.

Gudykunst, W. B., Ting-Toomey, S., & Chua, E. (1988). Culture and inter-personal communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). Culture and attachment:

Perceptions of the child in context. New York: Guilford.

Heine, S.

J.,

Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, '[, Ide, E., Leung, c., et al.

(2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves.

journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599-615.

Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and self-affirmation.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23,389-400.

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766-794.

Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children's intrin-sic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 774-795.

R. D., Kashiwagi, K., Azuma, H., Price, G. G., & Dickson, W. P. (1980).

Maternal expectations for mastery of developmental tasks in Japan and the United States. International journal of Psychology, 15,259-271.

Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motiva-tional principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-ogy (Vo!. 30, pp. 1-46). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psy-chologist, 55, 1217-1230.

Higgins, E. T., & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for self-regulation: A motivated cognition perspective. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 171-187). New York: Guilford.

Ho, D. Y. F. (1986). Chinese patterns of socialization: A critical review. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 1-37). Oxford:

Oxford University Press.

Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emo-tion regulaemo-tion. New York: Springer.

Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 76, 349-366.

Ji, L.-J., Nisbett, R. & Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psycho-logical Science, 12, 4S0-456.

John, O. P., & Gross, J . .J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Per-sonality processes, individual differences, and life span development. journal of Personality, 72, 1301-1333.

115 Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Kuensemueller, P., Abels, M., Yovsi, R. D., Voelker, S., et

al. (2004). The bio-culture of parenting: Evidence from five cultural communi-ties. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 25-50.

Kim, H. S., & Markus, :rI. R. (2002). Freedom of speech and freedom of silence:

An analysis of talking as a cultural practice. In R. A. Shweder, M. Minow, &

H. R. Markus (Eds.), Engaging cultural differences: The multicultural challenge in liberal democracies (pp. 432-452). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

King, L. A., Hicks,

J.

A., Krull,

J.

L., & Gaiso, A. K. (2006). Positive affect and the of meaning in Iife . .Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,179-196.

Kitayama, S. (2001). Culture and emotion. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 3134-3139). Oxford: Elsevier.

Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (Eds.) (1994). Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-tion.

Kitayama, 5., & Markus, H. R. (1995). Culture and self: Implications for interna-tionalizing psychology. In N. R. Goldberger & J. B. Veroff (Eds.), The culture and psychology reader (pp. 366-383). New York: New York University Press.

Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (2000). The pursuit of happiness and the realiz-ation of sympathy: Cultural patterns of self, social relrealiz-ations, and well-being. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and sub;ective well being (pp. 113-161).

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kitayama, 5., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition & Emo-tion, 14,93-124.

Kitayama, 5., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Indi-vidual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan . .Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245-1267.

Kochanska, G. (1998). Mother-child relationship, child fearfulness, and emerg-ing attachment: A short-term longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34,480-490.

Kuczynski, L. (Ed.). (2003). Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., & Singeiis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem . .Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1038-1051.

Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). Attachment and self-organization. In M. D. Lewis & 1. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization:

Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 298-323). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lehra, .[ S. (1994). Mother and child in Japanese socialization: A Japan-lJ. S.

comparison. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 259-274). HiIlsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Lee, A. Y., Aaker,

J.

L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and pains of

1

distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1122-1134.

Lehman, D. R., Chiu, c., & Schaller, M. (2004). Psychology and culture. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 689-714.

LeVine, R. A. (1988). Human parental care: Universal goals, cultural strategies, individual behavior. In R. A. LeVine & P. M. Miller (Eds.), New directions for child development: Vol. 40. Parental behavior in diverse societies (pp. 3-12). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

LeVine, R. A., & Miller, P. M. (1990). Commentary. Human Development, 33, 73-80.

Lewis, C. C. (1989). From indulgence to internalization: Social control in the early school years. Journal ofJapanese Studies, 15, 139-157.

Lewis, M. D. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38, 71-102.

Li, J. (2002). A cultural model of learning: Chinese "heart and mind for wanting to learn." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 248-269.

Lin, C.

c.,

& Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Develop-ment, 61,429-433.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cog-nition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98,224-253.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). The cultural construction of self and emo-tion: Implications for social behavior. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 89-130).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman & J. Berman Lincoln (Eds.), The 49th Annual Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Cross-cultural differ-ences in perspectives on self (pp. 1-57). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Markus, H. R., MullaiJy, P. R., & Kitayama, S. (1997). Selfways: Diversity in modes of cultural participation. In U. Neisser & D. A. Jopling (Eds.), The con-ceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp. 13-61). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Mason, C. A., Walker-Barnes, C. J., Tu, S., Simons, J., & Martinez-Arrue, R.

(2004). Ethnic differences in the affective meaning of parental control behav-iors. Journal of Primary Prevention, 25, 59-79.

McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2004). Self-conscious emotions as dynamic cul-tural processes. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 161-166.

Mesquita, B., & Markus, H. R. (2004). Culture and emotion: Models of agency as sources of cultural variation in emotion. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, &

A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium (pp. 341-358). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratifi-cation: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106,3-19.

117 Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect

regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attach-ment-related strategies. Motivation & Emotion, 27, 77-102.

Miller, P.

J.,

Wang, S., Sandel, T., & Cho, G. E. (2002). Self-esteem as folk theory:

A comparison of European American and Taiwanese mothers' beliefs. Parent-ing: Science and Practice, 2, 209-239.

Miller, P.

J.,

Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C.-H. (1997). Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Develop-ment, 68, 557-568.

Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective process-ing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In R. F. Baumeister &

K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applica-tions (pp. 99-129). New York: Guilford.

Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., Oppenheim, D., & Goldsmith, D. (1992). Cultural variation in infants' sleeping arrangements: Questions of independence. Devel-opmental Psychology, 28, 604-613.

Morelli, G. A., & Rothbaum, F. (2007). Situating the person in relationships: At-tachment relationships and self-regulation in young children. In S. Kitayama &

D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 500-527). New York:

Guilford.

Mulder, N. (1992). Individual and society in Java: A cultural analysis. Yo-gyakarta: Gadjah Mada University Press.

Norasakkunkit, V. (2003). Self-construal priming and emotional distress: Testing for cultural biases in the concept of distress. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Boston.

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individual-ism and collectivindividual-ism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses.

Psychological Bulletin, 128,3-72.

Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Roopnarine, J. L., & Carter, D. B. (Eds.) (1992). Parent-child socialization in diverse cultures. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates,

J.

(1998). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Hand-book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-ment (5th ed., pp. 105-176). New York: Wiley.

Rothbaum, E, & Kakinuma, M. (2004). Amae and attachment: Security in cul-tural context. Human Development, 47, 34-39.

Rothbaum, E, & MoreHi, G. (2005). Attachment and culture: Bridging relativism and universalism. In W. Friedlmeier, P. Chakkarath, & B. Schwarz (Eds.), Cul-ture and human development: The importance of cross-cultural research for the social sciences (pp. 99-123). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Rothbaum, E, Nagaoka, R., & Ponte, I. C. (2006). Caregiver sensitivity in cul-tural context: Japanese and U. S. teachers' beliefs about anticipating and re-sponding to children's needs. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 23-39.

Rothbaum, E, Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. R. (2000). The

[1

development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of sym-biotic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71, 1121-1142.

Rothbaum, E, & Trommsdorff, G. (2007). Do roots and wings complement or oppose one another? The socialization of relatedness and autonomy in cultural context. In J. E. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), The handbook of socialization (pp. 461-489). New York: Guilford.

Rothbaum, E, Weisz, J. R., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G. (2000). Attach-ment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan. American Psycholo-gist, 55,1093-1104.

Rubin, K. H. (1998). Social and emotional development from a cultural perspec-tive. Developmental Psychology, 34, 611-615.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychol-ogist, 55, 68-78.

Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New York: Guilford.

Saarni, C. (2001). Epilogue: Emotion communication and relationship context.

International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 354-356.

Shek, D. T. L. (2001). Chinese adolescents and their parents' views on a happy family: Implications for family therapy. Family Therapy, 28,73-104.

Shouldice, A. E., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1992). Coping with security distress:

The Separation Anxiety Test and attachment classification at 4.5 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 331-348.

Shweder, R. A., Goodnow,J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H. R., & Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many

Shweder, R. A., Goodnow,J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H. R., & Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE