• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

9.2 “The fundamental things apply As time goes by”

9.4 The Role of Presentation Modality

cality effects that manifest in increased reading times in the following regions. On the one hand, this indicates that both syntactic structures adversely affect sentence processing. On the other hand, the data also show expectation-based effects. For example, reading times of the matrix verb differed strongly in-between the condi-tions. However, the data for the region MV yield both locality- and anti-locality effects. Those findings fit to other reports that suggest a co-occurrence of locality-and anti-locality in sentence processing. The data of Experiment 5 suggest that beneficial effects might occur up to a certain length or a certain level of sentence complexity. However, if this level of complexity has been crossed, beneficial effects seem to dissolve. Further additional length increases complexity which manifests in locality effects.

Considerations of a co-occurrence of both expectations-based facilitation and distance-based locality effects also fits results of Experiment 6. It is suggested that complex and infrequent sentence constructions are more likely to become subject to locality than simple structures which often might benefit from additional length. Experiment 6 reveals clear-cut locality effects. However, a tendency in the data suggests that some critical and complex structures (ungrammatical sentences and garden-path sentences) are more sensitive to additional length than others.

Previous findings have shown that those conditions are more error prone than other conditions. Thus, the data support considerations that complex structures are more likely to be adversely affected by additional length than simple structures.

Furthermore, another important finding reported in this thesis concerns the experimental self-paced reading paradigm. Unexpected results of Experiment 4 indicate that non-linguistic information significantly affected the parsing process.

As discussed in chapter 7, the information about the number of words per sentence (which was unwillingly provided by the number of underlines on the screen) had a significant impact on the participants’ parsing processes. Modifying the exper-imental design of Experiment 4 in the follow-up Experiment 5 resulted in a lack of unwanted length effects. This supports the hypothesis that unwillingly pro-vided non-linguistic information is the underlying source of the significant and unwanted length effects.

9.4 The Role of Presentation Modality

The final core question of this thesis was concerned with effects of presentation modality on sentence processing. A wide range of studies from the list learning lit-erature concordantly report a superiority effect of auditory presented information.

Results show that visually presented items are recalled less accurate than lists that were presented auditory. As discussed above, this phenomenon is found in vari-ety of studies with letters, words or numbers. Some hypotheses suggest that the

specialized subcomponent of the working memory framework which is responsi-ble for language processing, bases on a phonological code. Thus, a recoding of visual information into this phonological code yields a processing disadvantage compared to already presented auditory material.

The final study in this thesis investigated, if complex cognitive tasks, such as sentence processing, also reveal the modality effect. Indeed, the main findings of Experiment 7 show a superiority effect of auditory presentation over visually pre-sented material. Those findings suggest that the effect reported from studies with unrelated lists can also be found in experiments on sentence processing. It seems that complex cognitive tasks are observably sensitive to presentation modality.

However, in contrast to predictions the data only partly show that critical sen-tences are more sensitive to presentation modality than other sentence conditions.

First, ungrammatical sentences are not affected by modality at all. Second, acc-initial ambiguous sentences only show some numerical advantage of auditory sen-tences over visual sensen-tences.

Altogether, the results of Experiment 7 reveal a superiority effect of the audi-tory presentation modality for complex cognitive tasks. However, it is suggested that future work investigates the issue of modality effects in sentence processing.

The results of experiments work presented in this dissertation yield some valu-able and interesting findings that contribute to a range of long-lasting, central and current questions in psycholinguistic research.

Bibliography

Adams, D. (1982). Life, the Universe and Everything. Pan Books, UK.

Altmann, G. and Y. Kamide (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restrict-ing the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73, 247–264.

Andrade, J. (Ed.) (2008). Memory – Critical Concepts in Psychology. Routledge, New York.

Ashcraft, M. H. (1995). Human Memory and Cognition. HarperCollins College Publisher, New York.

Atkinson, R. C. and R. M. Shiffrin (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychol-ogy of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Volume 2, pp. 89–195. New York: Academic Press.

Babyonyshev, M. and E. Gibson (1999). The complexity of nested structures in japanese. Language 75, 423–450.

Baddeley, A. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a fundction of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Psychology 18, 362–365.

Baddeley, A. (1976). The psychology of memory. Basic Books.

Baddeley, A. (1999). Essentials of Human Memory. Psychology Press, Hove.

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?

Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11), 417–423.

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. Journal of Communication Disorders 36, 189–208.

Baddeley, A. (2007). Working Memory, Thought, and Action. Oxford Psychology Press.

169

Baddeley, A., N. Thomson, and A. Buchanan (1975). Word length and the struc-ture of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14, 575–589.

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove: Psychology Press.

Baddeley, A. D. and G. Hitch (1974). Working Memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 8, pp. 47–90. New York: Academic Press.

Bader, M. and J. Bayer (2006). Case and linking in language comprehension:

Evidence from German. Dordrecht: Springer.

Bader, M., M. Meng, and J. Bayer (2000). Case and reanalysis. Journal of Psy-cholinguistic Research 29, 37–52.

Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structure. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language, pp. 279–362. New York:

Wiley and Sons.

Broadbent, D. E. (1957). A mechanical model for human attention and immediate memory. Psychological Review 64, 205–215.

Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quaterlay Journal of Experimental Psychology 10, 12–21.

Burgess, N. and G. J. Hitch (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review 106, 551–581.

Carroll, J. B., A. S. Carton, and C. P. Wilds (1959). An Investigation of "Cloze"

Items in the Measurement of Achievement in Foreign Languages. Cambridge, MA: Graduate School of Deducation, Harvard University, Laboratory for Re-search in Instruction.

Chen, E., E. Gibson, and F. Wolf (2005). Online syntactic storage costs in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 52, 144–169.

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

BIBLIOGRAPHY 171 Chomsky, N. and G. A. Miller (1963). Introduction to the formal analysis of nat-ural languages. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, and E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology. Vol. II, pp. 269–321. New York, London & Sidney:

Wiley.

Clifton, Charles, J. and L. Frazier (1989). Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In G. N. Carlson and M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing, pp. 273–317. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Colle, H. and A. Welsh (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior 15, 17–32.

Conrad, R. (1960). Very brief delay of immediate recall. Quaterlay Journal of Experimental Psychology 12, 45–47.

Conrad, R. (1964). Accoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology 55, 75–84.

Craik, F. I. and R. Lockhart (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for mem-ory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11, 671–684.

Crocker, M. W. (1999). Mechanisms for sentence processing. In S. Garrod and M. J. Pickering (Eds.), Language processing, Chapter 7, pp. 191–232. Psychol-ogy Press, Hove.

Crowder, R. and J. Morton (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (pas). Percep-tion and Psychophysics 5, 365–373.

Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-baum.

Daneman, M. and P. A. Carpenter (1980). Individual differences in working mem-ory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19, 450–466.

Draaisma, D. (2000). Metaphors of Memory. University Press, Cambridge.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das Gedächtnis. Duncker & Humblot, Leipzig.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1908). Abriss der Psychologie. Veit & Comp., Leipzig.

Farrell, S. and S. Lewandowsky (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9, 59–79.

Frazier, L. and J. Clifton, Charles (1989). Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Language and Cognitive Processes 4, 93–126.

Frazier, L. and K. Rayner (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sen-tences. Cognitive Psychology 14, 178–210.

Frisch, S. and M. Schlesewsky (2001). The N400 reflects problems of thematic hierarchizing. NeuroReport 12, 3391–3394.

Garrod, S. C. and A. J. Sanford (1982). The mental representation of dicourse in a focussed memory system: implications for the interpretation of anaphoric noun phrases. Journal of Semantics 1, 21–41.

Garrod, S. C. and A. J. Sanford (1994). Resolving sentences in a discourse con-text: How discourse representation affects language understanding. In M. A.

Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, pp. 675–698. San Diego etc.: Academic Press.

Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies.

Cognition 68, 1–76.

Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In A. Marantz, Y. Miyashita, and W. O’Neil (Eds.), Image, language, brain. Papers from the first Mind Articulation Project Sympo-sium, pp. 95–126. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gordon, P. C., R. Hendrick, and M. Johnson (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27, 1411–1423.

Gorrell, P. (Ed.) (1995). Syntax and parsing. Cambridge University Press, Cam-brige.

Grodner, D. and E. Gibson (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentential complexity. Cognitive Science 29, 261–290.

Haberlandt, K. (1999). Human Memory. Exploration and Application. Allyn &

Bacon.

Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. Pro-ceedings of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 159–166.

Hawkins, J. A. (1990). A parsing theory of word order universals. Linguistic Inquiry 21(2), 223–261.

BIBLIOGRAPHY 173 Hawkins, J. A. (2001). Why are categories adjacent? Journal of Linguistics 37,

1–34.

Henson, R. N. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model.

Cognitive Psychology 36, 73–137.

Jaeger, F. T. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (trans-formation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434–446.

Jaeger, F. T., E. Federenko, P. Hofmeister, and E. Gibson (2005). Expectation-based syntactic processing: Antilocality outside of head-final languages. In CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, North Carolina.

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. Macmillan and Co. Ltd., Lon-don.

Juola, J., N. Ward, and T. McNamara (1982). Visual search and reading rapid serial presentations of letter strings, words and text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111, 208–227.

Just, M. A. and P. A. Carpenter (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review 87, 329–354.

Just, M. A. and P. A. Carpenter (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension:

individual differences in working memory? Psychological Review 99, 122–

149.

Just, M. A., P. A. Carpenter, and T. A. Keller (1996). The capacity theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and arguments. Psychological Re-view 103, 773–780.

Just, M. A., P. A. Carpenter, and J. D. Wolley (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111, 228–238.

Kadmon, N. (2001). Formal Pragmatics. Blackwell Publisher.

Kaiser, E. and J. C. Trueswell (2004). The role of discourse context in the pro-cessing of a flexible word-order language. Cognition 94, 113–147.

Kamide, Y., G. T. Altmann, and S. Haywood (2000). Predictive eye-movements in incremental processing of head-final structures. In Thirteenth Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, La Jolla, California.

Kamp, H. (1984). A theory of truth and semantic representation. In J. Groe-nendijk, T. Janssen, and M. Stokhof (Eds.), Truth, Interpretation and Informa-tion., pp. 1–41. GRASS2, Foris.

Kaplan, R. M. (1972). Augmented Transition Networks as psychological models of sentence comprehension. Artificial Intelligence 3, 77–100.

Kimball, J. (1973). Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural lan-guage. Cognition 2, 15–48.

Konieczny, L. (1996). Human sentence processing: a semantics-oriented parsing approach (IIG-Bericht Nr. 3/96). Doctoral dissertation, Universität Freiburg.

Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguis-tic Research 29, 627–645.

Konieczny, L. and P. Döring (2003). Anticipation of clause-final heads: Evidence from eye-tracking and SRNs. In P. Slezak (Ed.), Proceedings of the 4th inter-national conference on cognitive science., pp. 330–335. Sydney: University of New South Wales.

Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition 106, 1126–1177.

Levy, R., E. Federenko, and E. Gibson (2007). The syntactic complexity of rus-sian relative clauses. In Paper presented at the Annual Conference on Human Sentence Processing CUNY.

Lewandowsky, S. (1999). Redintegration and response suppression in serial recall.

International Journal of Psychology 34, 434–446.

Lewandowsky, S., M. Duncan, and G. D. Brown (2004). Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11, 771–

790.

Lewandowsky, S. and J. Murdock, Bennet B. (1989). Memory for serial order.

Psychological Review 96, 25–58.

Lewandowsky, S., K. Oberauer, and G. D. Brown (2008). No temporal decay in verbal short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Science 13 (3), 120–126.

Lewis, R. and S. Vasishth (2005). An activation-based model of sentence process-ing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science 29, 375–419.

BIBLIOGRAPHY 175 Lewis, R. L. (1996). Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25, 93–115.

MacDonald, M. C., N. Pearlmutter, and M. S. Seidenberg (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101(4), 676–

703.

MacWhinney, B. and C. Pléh (1988). The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition 29, 95–141.

Marslen-Wilson, W. (1973). Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies. Nature 244, 522–523.

McGeoch, J. A. (1932). Forgetting and the law of disuse. Psychological Re-view 39, 352–370.

Melton, A. W. (1963). Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 2, 1–21.

Meng, M. (1998). Kognitive Sprachverarbeitung. Rekonstruktion syntaktischer Strukturen beim Lesen. Wiesbaden: Deutscher Universitätsverlag.

Meng, M. and M. Bader (2000). Ungrammaticality detection and garden-path strength: Evidence for serial parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes 15, 615–666.

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63, 81–97.

Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective. TRENDS in Cognitive Science 7(3), 141–144.

Miller, G. A. and N. Chomsky (1963). Finitary models of language users. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, and E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology. Vol. II, pp. 419–490. New York, London & Sidney: Wiley.

Mitchell, D. C. (1994). Sentence parsing. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, pp. 375–409. San Diego etc.: Academic Press.

Murdock, B. B. (1995). Developing todam. three models for serial-order informa-tion. Memory & Cognition 23, 631–645.

Nairne, J. S. (1990). A feature model of immediate memory. Memory & Cogni-tion 18, 251–269.

Nakatani, K. and E. Gibson (2010). An on-line study of japanese nesting com-plexity. Cognitive Science 34, 94 – 112.

Neath, I. (1999). Modelling the disruptive effects of irrelevant speech on order information. International Journal of Psychology 34, 410–418.

Page, M. P. and D. Norris (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review 105, 761–781.

Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term vrbal mem-ory. Memory & Cognition 17(4), 398–422.

Peterson, L. R. and M. J. Peterson (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology 58, 193–198.

Pickering, M. (1999). Sentence comprehension. In S. Garrod and M. Pickering (Eds.), Language processing, pp. 123–153. Hove: Psychology Press.

Pickering, M. and M. J. Traxler (2000). Parsing and incremental understand-ing durunderstand-ing readunderstand-ing. In M. W. Crocker, M. Pickerunderstand-ing, and J. Clifton, Charles (Eds.), Architectures and mechanisms for language processing, pp. 238–258.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Pickering, M. J. and M. J. Traxler (1998). Plausibility and recovery from garden paths: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 24, 940–961.

Roark, B. (2001). Probabilistic top-down parsing and language modeling. Com-putational Linguistics 27(2), 249–276.

Saito, S. and A. Miyake (2004). On the nature of forgetting and the processing-storage relationship in reading span performance. Journal of Memory and Lan-guage 20, 425–443.

Salamé, P. and A. Baddeley (1982). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 41A, 107–

122.

Schlesewsky, M., G. Fanselow, and S. Frisch (2003). Case as a trigger for reanal-ysis - some arguments from the processing of double case ungrammaticalities in german. In R. V. Susann Fischer, Ruben van de Vijer (Ed.), Linguistics in Potsdam, 21, pp. 31–60. Psychology Press, Hove.

BIBLIOGRAPHY 177 Shallice, T. and E. K. Warrington (1970). Independent functioning of verbal mem-ory stores: a neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 22, 261–273.

Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psy-chological Monographs: General and Applied 74 (11), 1–30.

Stolcke, A. (1997). Linguistic knowledge and empirical methods in speech recog-nition. AI Magazine 18 (4), 25 – 31.

Stromswold, K., D. Caplan, N. Alpert, and S. Rauch (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Lan-guage 52, 452–473.

Sturt, P. and M. W. Crocker (1998). Generalized monotonicity for reanalysis models. In J. D. Fodor and F. Ferreira (Eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing, pp. 365–400. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Tanenhaus, M. K., K. M. E. Michael J. Spivey-Knowlton, and J. C. Sedivy (1995).

Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehen-sion. Science 268, 1632–1634.

Taylor, W. (1953). Cloze procedure: A new tool for measuring readability. Jour-nalism Quarterly 30, 415–433.

Traxler, M. J., R. K. Morris, and R. E. Seely (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 47, 69–90.

Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving and W. Don-aldson (Eds.), Organization of memory., pp. 382–403. New York: Academic Press.

Van Dyke, J. A. and R. L. Lewis (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language 49, 285–316.

Vasishth, S., S. Bruessow, R. L. Lewis, and H. Drenhaus (2008). Processin gpo-larity: How the ungrammatical intrudes on the grammatical. Cognitive Sci-ence 32(4).

Vasishth, S. and H. Drenhaus (2011). Locality in German. Dialogue and Dis-course 1(2), 59–82.

Vasishth, S. and R. Lewis (2006). Argument-head distance and processing com-plexity: explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language 82, 767–

794.

Vasishth, S., K. Suckow, R. L. Lewis, and S. Kern (2010). Short-term forgetting in sentence comprehension: Crosslinguistic evidence from head-final structures.

Language and Cognitive Processes 25(4), 533–567.

Vosse, T. and G. Kempen (2000). Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing:

a computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist gram-mar. Cognition 75(2), 105–143.

Warren, T. and E. Gibson (1999). The effects of discourse status on intuitive complexity: Implications for quantifying distance in locality-based theory of linguistic complexity. Poster presented at the Twelth CUNY Sentence Process-ing Conference, CUNY, New York.

Waugh, N. C. and D. A. Norman (1965). Primary memory. Psychological Re-view 72, 89–104.

Winograd, T. (1972). Understanding natural language. Cognitive Psychology 3, 1–191.

Yates, F. A. (1966). The Art of Memory. Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Yngve, V. H. A. (1960). A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Pro-ceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104, 444–466.

BIBLIOGRAPHY 179 a

Experimental Methods A

All experiments presented in this thesis were run in thePSYCHOLINGUISTIC LAB

-ORATORY of the University of Konstanz. In this laboratory, experiments on lan-guage processing in German are run all-year-round. All subjects participating in the various studies are students with German as their native language who are recruited from the campus community. Participation in the experiments is re-warded with either a one hour credit (Versuchspersonenstunde) or a payment of 5-7 Euro per hour. Research priorities investigated in the laboratory focus on cur-rently debated aspects of speech processing from a syntactic point of view. The experiments make use of a wide range of experimental methods in both visual and auditory input. Self-paced reading studies (SPR), speeded grammaticality judg-ment tasks (SGJ) are amongst the used methodologies. All experijudg-ments in this doctoral thesis were accomplished with either the self-paced reading paradigm or the speeded grammaticality judgment paradigm. Both experimental tasks will be discussed briefly in this appendix.