• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

It is generally assumed that dative is assigned in a different projection than accusative, both for indirect objects of ditransitive verbs and for single objects of noncanonical case marking verbs (e.g., Meinunger 2006; McFadden 2006). Some accounts also suggest that upon encountering a dative verb, the lexical entry of the object NP has to be reaccessed to check for dative morphology, indepen-dently of the animacy of the NPs (this explanation was used by Hopf et al. 1998;

see also Bayer et al. 2001). This would mean that the parser has to restructure the syntactic representation of the sentence once a dative verb is encountered, and possibly reaccess the lexical entry of the object NP. Under this assumption, our results could be explained as reflecting the additional processing costs caused by the extra workload of rearranging the syntactic representation, and

of lexical reaccess to check for dative morphology. This process could be reflected in the negativities found after the presentation of the dative verbs in the left-posterior ROI (see Figure 3, panel B), and could be argued to replace or shift the processing of object animacy in the dative condition. This would mean that in the accusative conditions, the negativities represent a true animacy effect. In the dative conditions, however, the negativity is caused by a comple-tely different process that happens to also be reflected in a negativity. The fact that there is no animacy effect on the verb in dative conditions could mean that the animate objects are licensed by dative verbs (again, a more semantically flavored explanation), that the processing of object animacy differences is delayed until the case marking processing is finished, or else, that both pro-cesses work in parallel, but their effects on the EEG do not simply add up.

The results of the current study do not allow us to distinguish between these different explanations. Especially the distinction between the second (seman-tics-based) and third (syntax-based) explanations would be difficult to realize.

This has methodological as well as linguistic reasons. Since the non-canonical semantics and non-canonical syntax of dative verbs always go together, there are no dative-assigning verbs with prototypically transitive argument semantics in German. Therefore, the factors “canonical/noncanonical syntax”and“ cano-nical/noncanonical argument semantics”cannot be fully crossed. For potential experiments investigating more subtle semantic distinctions, stimulus construc-tion would be made difficult by the limited number of dative-assigning verbs in German. Since accusative and dative-assigning verbs have to be matched for frequency, length and the selectional restrictions concerning argument seman-tics (i.e., they all have to allow for animate subjects, and both animate and inanimate objects), this limited set of verbs is further restricted. For any more restricted subgroup of dative verbs, there simply would not be enough verbs left in the German language to build stimulus material for a sound EEG analysis without repeating the same verbs over and over again. While the semantics and syntax of dative verbs remain highly interesting, a new approach will be needed to disentangle the contribution of these two factors to the processing load associated with dative verbs.

5 Conclusions

In line with the literature, we found that animate objects cause higher proces-sing costs than inanimate objects in German transitive sentences without overt case marking or number congruency. We found that these effects of object animacy occur even if these sentences are grammatical. We show that the effect

of object animacy is modulated (i.e., reduced in the left-posterior ROI) if the verb of the sentence has a noncanonical case marking pattern. Noncanonical case marking patterns indicate nonprototypically transitive semantics with more agentive objects. Future research will have to address whether this modulation of the processing of object animacy reflects syntactic or semantic processing, differences in lexical access, or a combination of several factors.

Acknowledgements: We gratefully acknowledge the technical assistance of Carsten Schliewe.

References

Bader, Markus & Jana Häussler. 2010. Word order in German: A corpus study.Lingua120(3). 717762.

Bader, Markus, Michael Meng & Josef Bayer. 2000. Case and reanalysis 1.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research29. 3753.

Bayer, Josef, Markus Bader & Michael Meng. 2001. Morphological underspecification meets oblique case: Syntactic and processing effects in German.Lingua111. 465514.

Belletti, Adriana & Luigi Rizzi. 1988. Psych-verbs and theta-theory.Natural Language and Linguistic Theory6. 291352.

Blume, Kerstin. 2000.Markierte Valenzen im Sprachvergleich. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.

Bock, J. Kathryn. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production.Cognitive Psychology18.

355387.

Bock, J. Kathryn & Richard Warren. 1985. Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation.Cognition21. 4767.

Bornkessel, Ina, Brian McElree, Matthias Schlesewsky & Angela D. Friederici. 2004. Multi-dimensional contributions to garden path strength: Dissociating phrase structure from case marking.Journal of Memory and Language51. 495522.

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Ina & Matthias Schlesewsky. 2006. The extended argument depen-dency model: A neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages.

Psychological Review113. 787821.

Bossong, Georg. 1985.Empirische Universalienforschung: Differentielle Objektmarkierung in den neuiranischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Narr.

Bossong, Georg. 1991. Differential object marking in romance and beyond. In Dieter Wanner &

Douglas A. Kibbee (eds.),New analyses in romance linguistics, 143170. Amsterdam &

Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Andrew J. Stewart & Janet F. McLean. 2000. Syntactic priming in spoken production: Linguistic and temporal interference.Journal of Memory and Cognition28. 12971302.

Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering & Mikihiro Tanaka. 2008. Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production.Lingua118.

172189.

Brouwer, Harm, Hartmut Fitz & John Hoeks. 2012. Getting real about semantic illusions:

rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension.Brain research 1446. 127143.

Corbett, Greville G. 2000.Number. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Croft, William. 1990.Typology and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Czypionka, Anna. 2014.The interplay of object animacy and verb class in representation building. Berlin: Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin dissertation.

Dixon, Robert M. W. 1994.Ergativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dowty, David. 1991. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection.Language67. 547619.

Fanselow, Gisbert. 2000. Optimal exceptions. In Barbara Stiebels & Dieter Wunderlich (eds.), Lexicon in focus, 173209. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.

Ferreira, Fernanda. 1994. Choice of passive voice is affected by verb type and animacy.Journal of Memory and Language33. 715736.

Frazier, Lyn, Lori Taft, Tom Roeper, Charles Clifton & Kate Erlich. 1984. Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension.Journal of Memory and Cognition12.

421430.

Frisch, Stefan & Matthias Schlesewsky. 2001. The N400 reflects problems of thematic hier-archizing.NeuroReport12. 33913394.

Grewe, Tanja, Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Stefan Zysset, Richard Wiese, D. Yves Von Cramon

& Matthias Schlesewsky. 2007. The role of the posterior superior temporal sulcus in the processing of unmarked transitivity.NeuroImage35. 343352.

Grimm, Scott. 2010. Semantics of case.Morphology21. 515544.

Hagoort, Peter, Colin Brown & Jolanda Groothusen. 1993. The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing.Language and cognitive processes8(4). 439483.

Haspelmath, Martin. 2013. Occurrence of nominal plurality. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.),The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.

Heister, Julian, Kay-Michael Würzner, Johannes Bubenzer, Edmund Pohl, Thomas Hanneforth, Alexander Geyken & Reinhold Kliegl. 2011. dlexDBeine lexikalische Datenbank für die psychologische und linguistische Forschung.Psychologische Rundschau32. 1020.

Hoeks, John C.J., Laurie Stowe & Gina Doedens. 2004. Seeing words in context: The interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading.Cognitive Brain Research19. 5973.

Hopf, Jens-Max, Josef Bayer, Markus Bader & Michael Meng. 1998. Event-related brain poten-tials and case information in syntactic ambiguities.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience10.

264280.

Jäger, Gerhardt. 2004. Learning constraint sub-hierarchies: The bidirectional gradual learning algorithm. In Reinhard Blutner & Henk Zeevat (eds.),Optimality theory and pragmatics, 442501. Basingstoke: Palgrave McMillan.

Kim, Albert & Lee Osterhout. 2005. The independence of combinatory semantic processing:

Evidence from event-related potentials.Journal of Memory and Language52. 205225.

Krifka, Manfred. 2009. Case syncretism in German feminines: Typological, functional and structural aspects. In Manfred Krifka & Patrick Steinkrüger (eds.),On inflection, 141172.

Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Kuperberg, Gina R. 2007. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax.Brain Research1146. 2349.

Kuperberg, Gina R., Donna A. Kreher, Tatiana Sitnikova, David N. Caplan & Phillip J. Holcomb.

2007. The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sen-tences: Evidence from event-related potentials.Brain and Language100. 223237.

Kuperberg, Gina R., Tatiana Sitnikova, David N. Caplan & Phillip J. Holcomb. 2003.

Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences.Cognitive Brain Research17. 117129.

Kuperberg, Gina R., Tatiana Sitnikova, Mariana Eddy & Phillip J. Holcomb. 2006. Neural correlates of processing syntactic, semantic and thematic relationships in sentences.

Language and Cognitive Processes 21. 117129.

Kutas, Marta & Steven A. Hillyard. 1980. Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity.Science207(4427). 203205.

Kutas, Marta & Steven A. Hillyard. 1982. The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing.Neuropsychologia20. 579590.

Kutas, Marta, Cyma K. Van Petten & Robert Kluender. 2006. Psycholinguistics electrified II (19942005). In Matthew Traxler & Morton Gernsbacher (eds.),Handbook of psycholin-guistics, 2nd edn., 659724. New York: Academic Press.

Ledoux, Kerry, Matthew J. Traxler & Tamara Y. Swaab. 2007. Syntactic priming in comprehen-sion: Evidence from event-related potentials.Psychological Science18(2). 135143.

Levin, Beth. 1993.English verb classes and alternationsA preliminary investigation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Levy, Roger. 2008. Expectation-based syntactic comprehension.Cognition106. 11261177.

Malchukov, Andrej L. 2008. Animacy and asymmetries in differential case marking.Lingua118 (2). 203221.

McFadden, Thomas. 2004.The position of morphological case in the derivation: A study on the syntax-morphology interface. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania dissertation.

McFadden, Thomas. 2006. German inherent datives and argument structure. In Daniel Hole, André Meinunger & Werner Abraham (eds.),Datives and other cases, 4978. Amsterdam &

Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Meinunger, André. 2006. Remarks on the projection of dative arguments in German. In Daniel Hole, André Meinunger & Werner Abraham (eds.),Datives and other cases, 5079.

Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Meinunger, André. 2007. Der Dativ im DeutschenEine Verständnishilfe für das Phänomen der gespaltenen Ergativität.Linguistische Berichte209. 333.

Naess, Åshild 2004. What markedness marks: The markedness problem with direct objects.

Lingua114. 11861212.

Osterhout, Lee & Phillip J. Holcomb. 1992. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly.Journal of Memory and Language31(6). 785806.

Paczynski, Martin & Gina R. Kuperberg. 2009. The impact of grammatical voice and subject noun animacy on verb processing. Paper presented at the Neurobiology of Language Conference, University of Chicago, 1516 October.

Paczynski, Martin & Gina R. Kuperberg. 2011. Electrophysiological evidence for use of the animacy hierarchy, but not thematic role assignment, during verb-argument processing.

Language and Cognitive Processes26(9). 14021456.

Prat-Sala, Mercè & Holly P. Branigan. 2000. Discourse constraints on syntactic processing in language production: A cross-linguistic study in English and Spanish.Journal of Memory and Language42(2). 168182.

Sassenhagen, Jona, Matthias Schlesewsky & Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky. 2014. On incre-mental interpretation: Degrees of meaning accessed during sentence comprehension.

Brain and Language114. 2939.

Silverstein, M. 1976. Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Robert M. W. Dixon (ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages, 112171. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.

Trueswell, John C., Michael K. Tanenhaus & Susan M. Garnsey. 1994. Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution.Journal of Memory and Language33. 285318.

van Nice, Kathy Y. & Rainer Dietrich. 2003. Task sensitivity of animacy effects: Evidence from German picture descriptions.Linguistics41. 825849.

Wechsler, Stephen. 1995.The semantic basis of argument structure. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Weckerly, Jill & Marta Kutas. 1999. An electrophysiological analysis of animacy effects in the processing of object relative sentences.Psychophysiology36. 559570.

Woolford, Ellen. 2006. Lexical case, inherent case, and argument structure.Linguistic Inquiry 37. 111130.