• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Age and Gender: Similarly Cued from Silhouetted Face Profiles

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Age and Gender: Similarly Cued from Silhouetted Face Profiles"

Copied!
1
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Age and Gender: Similarly Cued from Silhouetted Face Profiles

Nicolas Davidenko (ndaviden@psych.stanford.edu) Michael Ramscar (michael@psych.stanford.edu)

Psychology Department, Stanford University Jordan Hall, Building 420, Stanford, CA 94305

Introduction

Our ability to recognize one another relies heavily on our capacity to extract identifying information from faces.

Since most faces share the same basic features and configurations, we must attend to more subtle differences to successfully distinguish among them.

Prior to identifying individual faces, we must be able to extract socially relevant information such as gender and age.

Wild et al. (2000) showed that people can identify gender from faces in the absence of sex-stereotyped cues. Here we further this claim and show that both gender and age can be determined from information-scarce images of faces. A linear model reveals that the cues we use to estimate age overlap with those we use for gender identification.

Method and Results Human ratings

Using a graphics editor, images of 48 face profiles (24 male, 24 female, ranging in age from 16 to 58 years old) were reduced to black & white silhouettes, as shown in Figure 1A. Undergraduate subjects (n = 383) each rated 16 of the faces on either approximate age (“teens”, “20s”, “30s”,

“40s”, “50s”, or “60s”) or gender (“male” or “female” with a 7-point confidence score). Ss who rated age were given either no gender information (Group 1, n=54) or correct gender information (Group 2, n=54). Ss who rated gender were either given no age information (Group 3, n=164) or informed incorrectly that all the faces were of 21-year-olds (Group 4, n=56) or 40-year-olds (Group 5, n=55).

Accuracy

When given no additional information, Ss correctly judged age and gender well above chance (p < .000001). Ss in Groups 1 and 2 estimated age to within one age bin 63.9%

and 58.9% of the time, respectively, with chance performance at 44%. Ss in Groups 3, 4, and 5 identified gender 69.5%, 69.2%, and 69.8% of the time, respectively, with chance performance at 50%.

Providing Ss with gender information affected their ratings of age, and vice versa. Labeling female faces as

“female” caused Ss to rate them as older, and labeling male faces as “male” caused Ss to rate them as younger. This suggests that some of the face cues indicative of youth may also be indicative of femininity. Consistently, labeling faces as “21-year-olds” resulted in more male ratings, though this effect was only significant for gender-ambiguous faces (as determined by Group 3). Labeling faces as “40-year-olds”

had no reliable effects.

Linear Model

All 48 faces were parameterized by normalizing their size and orientation and identifying 11 key points along their contours (Figure 1B). A principal components (PC) analysis was performed on the informative coordinates from the key points. This produced 18 independent variables for two linear regression models aimed to predict perceived age and gender. PCs 1, 4, 5, and 6 reliably predicted age, and PCs 1, 4, and 9 reliably predicted perceived gender. PCs 1 and 4 were positively correlated with age and masculinity.

Figure 1. An example of a silhouetted face profile (A) and the 11 key points used for the parameterization (B).

Discussion

Results from human raters and the linear models suggest that similar cues from silhouetted face profiles predict age and gender. Specifically, face features that correlate with youth also correlate with femininity, and features that correlate with old age also correlate with masculinity. The parameterization method described above can be used to extract other socially relevant information from face profiles, such as race, attractiveness, expression, familiarity, and distinctiveness, as well as to construct “morphed” faces that vary arbitrarily in any of these attributes. We are currently examining how these attributes are inter-correlated and what this may reveal about human face representation.

Acknowledgments

This work has benefited from conversations with Josh Tenenbaum, Tom Griffiths, Mark Steyvers, and Dan Yarlett.

References

Wild H., Barrett S., Spence M., O’Toole A., Chenh Y., &

Brooke J. (2002). Recognition and sex classification of adults’ and children’s faces: examining performance in the absence of sex-stereotyped cues. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 269-291.

1334

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

The common feature of the smile line characteristics was that younger females (&lt; 35 years) presented with higher smile lines than older males (&gt; 35 years), a fact which was

Therefore, we defined three independent features describing the observer: qualification, gender, and age and two indepen- dent features describing the image: the level of

It can be shown that we will obtain the same results as before when we relax the assumption of separate job markets for men and women if individuals expect to obtain

Or you could discuss a methodological or theoretical debate within gender and development (e.g., North-South power imbalances, how to measure empowerment, etc.) If you take

The visual construction of femininities and masculinities; how visual culture both reflects and produces ideologies of gender; the unearthing of neglected artists,

We reconstruct COVID-19 infection rates by age and sex from officially reported data for ten European countries (Belgium, Czechia, Denmark, Germany, Italy, Norway, Portugal,

The magnitude of gender effects in the middle age group reached a meaningful level only in need-of-help recognition tasks, no dif- ferences between boys and girls regarding RTs

More specifically, we will take occupational status and tenure as potential moderators for the age/RTC relationship into account, since employees with more autonomy in their jobs