• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Taiji practice attenuates psychobiological stress reactivity : a randomized controlled trial in healthy subjects

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Taiji practice attenuates psychobiological stress reactivity : a randomized controlled trial in healthy subjects"

Copied!
10
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Taiji practice attenuates psychobiological stress reactivity - A randomized controlled trial in healthy subjects

Marko Nedelj kovic a,*, Brigitte Ausfeld-Hafter

a,

Konrad S treitberger

b,

Roland Seiler c, Petra H. Wirtz d

a University of Bern, Institute of Complementary Medidne KIKOM, lmhoof·Pavillon, lnse/spital, CH-3010 Bern, Switzerland

b lnselspital Bem, University Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Therapy, CH-3010 Bern, Switzerland

c University of Bern, Institute of Spart Sdence, Alpeneggstrasse 22, CH-3012 Bern, Switzerland

d University of Bern, Department of Psychology, Biological and Health Psychology, Alpeneggstrasse 22, CH-3012 Bern, Switzerland

KEYWORDS Taiji;

Psychosocial stress;

Salivary cortisol;

Salivary a-amylase;

Heart rate

Summary

Background: Stress reducing effects of Taiji, a mindful and gentle form of body movement, have been reported in previous studies, but standardized and controlled experimental studies are scarce. The present study investigates the effect of regular Taiji practice on psychobiological stress response in healthy men and women.

Methods: 70 partidpants were randomly assigned to either Taiji classes or a waiting list. After 3 months, 26 (8 men, 18 women) persons in the Taiji group and 23 (9 men, 14 women) in the waiting control group underwent a standardized psychosocial stress test combining public speaking and mental arithmetic in front of an audience. Salivary cortisol and a-amylase, heart rate, and psychological responses to psychosodal stress were compared between the study groups. (ClinicalTrials.gov number, NCT01122706.)

Results: Stress induced characteristic changes in all psychological and physiological mea.sures.

Compared to controls, Taiji partidpants exhibited a significantly lower stress reactivity of cortisol (p = .028) and heart rate (p = .028), as well as lower a-amylase levels (p = .049). They reported a lower increase in perceived stressfulness (p = .006) and maintained a higher level of calmness ( p = . 019) in response to psychosodal stress.

Conclusion: Our results consistently suggest that practidng Taiji attenuates psychobiological stress reactivity in healthy subjects. This may underline the role of Taiji as a useful mind-body practice for stress prevention.

• Corresponding author. Tel.: +41 31 632 9758; fax: +41 31 632 4162.

E-mail address: marko.nedeljkovic@kikom.unibe.ch (M. Nedeljkovic).

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-0-276658

(2)

1. Introduction

Theharmfulimpactofstressonhealthhasbeendocumented repeatedly(Rozanskietal.,1999;Ehlertetal.,2001;Brot- man etal.,2007; Shonkoff etal.,2009). Stress reactivity researchsuggeststhatadverseconsequencesofpsychosocial stresson physicalandmentalhealth mayrelateto stress- inducedactivationofdifferentstress-responsivephysiologi- cal systems (Ehlert etal., 2001; Lovallo and Gerin, 2003;

RaisonandMiller,2003;Brotmanetal.,2007).Large-magni- tudephysiologicalreactionstoacutestressorsinparticular, oftencombinedwithdelayedrecovery,couldbeidentifiedas stress-relatedriskfactorsforcardiovasculardisease(Steptoe etal., 2006; Chida and Hamer, 2008; Chida and Steptoe, 2010).Moreprecisely,stress-inducedhyperreactivityofthe two main human stress systems, the hypothalamus—pitui- tary—adrenal(HPA)axiswithitsend-productcortisolandthe sympatheticnervoussystem (SNS)mayincreasecardiovas- cularrisk,eitheraloneand/orbyinducingadversechangesin intermediate biological risk factors for cardiovascular dis- ease such as coagulation activity, inflammation, or lipids (Rosmond and Bjorntorp, 2000; von Kanel et al., 2001;

Lovallo and Gerin, 2003; Rozanski et al., 2005; Brotman et al., 2007; Steptoe et al., 2007; Chida and Steptoe, 2010;Hameretal.,2010).

Thesefindingsunderlinetheimportanceofinvestigating stresspreventiveinterventionsandtheireffectsonpsycho- biological stress reactivity. By now, cognitive behavioural stressmanagementhasbeenrepeatedlyshowntomarkedly decrease psychological and biological reactivity towards psychosocialstressin healthysubjects (Gaab etal.,2003;

Hammerfaldetal.,2006).Incontrast,theeffectivenessof mind—body interventions for reducing psychosocial stress reactivity so far has only been examined for compassion meditation, showing a dose dependent effect on immune andpsychologicalstressresponses(Paceetal.,2009).

Mind—bodypracticesarecharacterizedasmethodsfocus- ingon the interactions among the brain,mind, body,and behaviour,withtheintentofusingthemindtoaffectphysical functioningandpromotinghealth(U.S.NationalInstitutesof Health, 2010). In fact, an increasing amount of scientific evidencesuggeststhatmind—bodypractices,suchasTaiji, might contributeto improvementsin physical and mental health(KleinandAdams, 2004;Jahnkeetal.,2010;Wang etal.,2010a,b).Taiji—variablyspelledTaijiquan,TaiChior TaiChiChuan—isamindfulandgentleformofslowbody movements with roots in ancient Chinese martial arts.

Becauseofits integrationofnumerousphysical,cognitive, andcontextualcomponentswhichpotentiallyhaveindepen- dentaswellassynergistictherapeuticvalue,Taijihasbeen describedasacomplexmulti-componentmind—bodyprac- tice(WayneandKaptchuk,2008).

AfewstudiessuggestthatTaijimayhavestressreducing effects.TwostudiessuggestthatpracticingTaijihasshort- andlong-termeffectsonthebasalactivityoftheHPA-axis.In hispioneeringresearchwork,Jin(1989)foundthatpracti- cingTaiji for60min reduced cortisol levels after Taiji as comparedtobefore.Similarly,resultsfromanon-controlled pilotstudyfoundreducedsalivarycortisollevelsinhealthy subjects, bothimmediately and 4weeks after they com- pletedaTaijibeginnerscourse(Eschetal.,2007).Hitherto,

studiesexploringthesuitabilityofTaijiasastressmanage- mentinterventionarescarce.Intermsofstressasmeasured bypsychologicalmeasuresonly,adecreaseofself-reported stress was observedin healthy youngadults as wellas in elderlysubjectswithcardiovasculardiseaseriskfactorsand personswithHIVdisease(Robinsetal.,2006;Taylor-Piliae etal.,2006;Eschetal.,2007).Todate,onerandomized- controlledstudyassessedtheeffectof60minofTaijiprac- ticeonthepsychobiologicalrecoveryofsubjectsafterthey wereexposedtoanon-validatedstressorintendedtoinduce mentalandemotionalstressbyhavingthemwatchstressful moviesandperformmentalarithmeticundertime—pressure andnoise (Jin, 1992). Adecrease of salivary cortisol was measuredafterTaijiaswellasafterthreedifferentinter- ventions(reading,briskwalking,meditation).However,due tolimited saliva sampling andmissing pre-stressbaseline measurement,findingsfromthisstudyremaininconclusive.

Taken together, studies measuring self-reported psycho- metricparametersconsistentlysuggestthatTaijimayserve asaneffectivestressmanagementinterventiontechnique, but its effects on physiological reactivity to acute stress remainunclear.

Tothebestofourknowledge,randomizedcontrolledtrials examiningtheeffectsof Taijion physiologicalandpsycho- logicalreactivitytostandardizedpsychosocialstressorshave notyet beenreported. Wethus setoutto investigatethe effectsofTaijion psychobiologicalreactivityto astandar- dizedandwell-validatedstressor,theTrierSocialStressTest (TSST).Werepeatedlyassesseddifferentmeasuresofinde- pendent stress responsive systems such as self-reported stressfulness,mood,andcalmness,aswellasthephysiolo- gical stress indicators salivary cortisol, salivary a-amylase levelsandheartrate.WehypothesizedthatpracticingTaiji wouldbeassociatedwithlowerpsychobiologicalstressreac- tivity.

2. Methods

2.1. Participantsanddesign

The ethics committeeof the Canton of Bern, Switzerland formallyapproved theresearchprotocol.Recruitmentwas carried outfromApril2010 toAugust2010 throughadver- tisementofthestudyonpinboardsandonthewebsitesofthe UniversityofBernandtheUniversityHospitalinBern.

Throughtelephonescreening,healthysubjectsagedfrom 18 to 50years and fluent in Germanwere included ifthe followingexclusioncriteriadidnotapplywithinthe6months priortothescreening(yes/no):regularoroccasionalintake of any medication, any self-reported acute or chronic somaticormentaldisorders,smokingmorethanfivecigar- ettes/day,consumptionofmorethantwoalcoholicdrinks/

day, consuming anykind ofaddictive substances,any pre- vious participationin stress researchprojects (in order to ensurethatsubjectsincluded werenaı¨ve totheTSST pro- tocol),morethan1weekofpredictableabsenceduringthe interventionperiod,anypreviouspracticalexperiencewith Taiji exercises. Women who were using hormonal contra- ceptives, were pregnant or planning to become pregnant duringthestudywerealsoexcluded.Theincludedsubjects receivedcompletewrittenandoraldescriptionsofthestudy.

(3)

Informedwrittenconsentwasobtainedbeforeparticipating.

Afterbaselineexaminationwascompleted,theparticipants were randomly assigned to either the Taiji group or the waitingcontrolgroup.Theallocationratiowas1:1.Alloca- tion concealmentwas achievedbyusingsequentially num- bered, opaque and sealed envelopes. An independent researchassistantgeneratedtherandomallocationsequence by sealing,mixing andsubsequently numbering80 opaque envelopes. Theywere opened individually by the primary investigator(MN)foreacheligiblesubjectwhohadagreedto participate in thestudy andcompleted baseline examina- tion.TSSTexaminationwascompletedonlyonsubjectswith compliance to start andtest instructions. The participant inclusionprocessisdepictedinFig.1.

2.2. Taijiintervention

The Taiji course being offered to the intervention group started in September 2010 and lasted for 12weeks. The training sessions of 60min took place twice a week. Taiji classesdifferedincomposition(participantschose2—6poten- tialtrainingtimepointsperweek)andsize(5—15participants persession).Participantswhomissedaclasswere askedto attendamake-upclass.Theinterventiongroupwasencour- agedtopracticeTaijiathomeinadditiontotheclasses.The averagenumberofhomepracticesessionswasassessedretro- spectivelyusingabriefself-reportquestionnaireattheendof thecourse.Participants’classattendancewasjournalizedby the Taiji teacher. All classes were held by the same Taiji teacher. Hewas trainedinChina aswell asin Europe,has 10yearsofTaijiexperience,andisacertifiedTaijiteacheras

awardedbytheSwissSocietyforQigongandTaijiquan(Schwei- zerischeGesellschaftfu¨rQigongundTaijiquan—SGQT).

IntheTaijicourse,participantsweretaughtthefirst18 sequencesofthe37ChenMan-Ch’ingYang-StyleTaijishort form.AnadaptationoffivesimplifiedTaijimovementsfrom thisformhasbeenpreviouslyusedinTaijitrialsonpatients with chronicheart failure (Yehetal.,2004,2011). Asour studyparticipantswereallhealthy,wedecidednottosim- plifytheformbuttoteachthefirst18movementsconsecu- tively, as recommended by Robinson (2006). The main reasonsforchoosingthisformarethefollowing:(1)inclusion ofthebasicTaijiprinciplessuchasextension,relaxationand alignmentofthebody,aswellasholisticandmindfulbody movements (Wolf et al., 1997), (2) feasibility given the moderateteachingandpracticingtimeof2hperweekfor 3months,(3)enhancedembodimentofbasicTaijiprinciples thanksto frequentrepetitionsenabledbytheshortness of the18sequences.Moreover,theChengMan-Ch’ingform is widely taught in Switzerland and subjects interested in learningtheremainingpartofthisformafterthestudywould easily find asuitable Taiji-school.Each Taijisession began withwarm-upexercises(15min)followedbypracticingTaiji movementsandreviewingtheunderlyingprinciples(35min) and concluded with Taijirelated breathing andrelaxation exercises(10min).

Priortogroupallocationparticipantsofbothstudygroups wererequestednottotakepartinanynewphysicalexercise or mind—bodyprogrammeduringtheirstudyparticipation.

Allparticipantsagreedwiththisrequest.Afterthetermina- tionofthestudy,anequivalentTaijitrainingwasofferedto allsubjectsparticipatinginthecontrolgroup.

Figure1 Flowdiagramfortheprogressthroughthephasesoftherandomizedtrial(basedontheconsolidatedstandardsofreporting trials[CONSORT]recommendations).Documentationofthesubjectinclusionprocess.

(4)

2.3. Assessment ofpotentiallyconfounding variables

2.3.1. PotentialTaiji-relatedconfoundingvariable Weassessedparticipants’previousregularpracticalexperi- ence(inmonths)withself-applicablemind—bodypractices (i.e.meditation,Feldenkrais,AlexanderTechnique,Qigong, Yoga, Pilates, guided imagery, deep breathing exercises, progressivemusclerelaxationandReiki)atbaselinetorule outanon-Taijirelatedinfluenceofpriormind—bodypractice experienceontheparametersunderstudy.

2.3.2. Potentialconfoundersofphysiologicalstress reactivity

Wecontrolledforage(Kudielkaetal.,2004),aswellasfor menstrualcyclephase(lutealvs.follicularphase,seebelow) andgenderassalivarycortisolreactivityinhormonalcontra- ceptive-freefemalesubjectsisbluntedduringthefollicular phaseanddiffers from cortisolreactivity in male subjects (Kirschbaumetal.,1999).Weaskedallfemaleparticipantsto fillouta questionnaireassessing duration(days) andregu- larityofthemenstrualcyclephase(yes/no),aswellasthe datesofonset ofmenstruationbefore andafterthestress testexamination.Lutealphasewasdefinedasthetimespan of14daysbeforeonsetofmenstruation(Lentonetal.,1984).

Additionally,wecontrolledforthecardiovascularriskfactors smoking(number of cigarettessmoked per day) andbody massindex(BMI,kg/m2,seeTable1),aswellasforregular physicalactivity(averagehoursperweek)duringtheinter- vention period (Rohleder and Kirschbaum, 2006; Rimmele etal.,2007;Bensonetal.,2009).

2.4. ProcedureoftheTrier SocialStressTest (TSST)examination

Theexperimental sessionswereconductedduringthefirst 3weeksafterterminationofthe12weekTaijiintervention between1300h and 1800h. The timing of the stress test

performancewasbalancedbetweenmalesandfemalesand betweenparticipantsinthetwo studygroups.Participants weretoldto refrainfrom eatinganddrinkinganythingbut waterfor 2hand from intense physical activity,caffeine, nicotine,andalcoholduringthe24hbeforetheexperiment.

Participants’ compliance to preparatory instructions and absenceoftheexclusioncriteriawasverified;non-compliant participants were excluded from the TSST. Next,the ECG recordingequipment was attached and the recordingwas started.WeusedtheTrierSocialStressTest(TSST)combining a 10min preparation phase followedby a5min mock job interview,anda 5min mentalarithmeticexercise (Kirsch- baumetal.,1993).Bothtaskswereperformed2minfrontof twoevaluativepanelmembersdressedinwhitelaboratory coats,andaconspicuousvideocameraandmicrophone.The socio-evaluative character of this performance task was further underlined by presenting the panel members (a retired male finance manager and a female psychologist) as experts inevaluationof nonverbal behaviour.The TSST reliably activates HPA-axis and the sympathetic nervous system(DickersonandKemeny,2004).Duringrecovery,sub- jectsremainedseatedinaquietroomfor60min.

2.5. Outcomemeasures

All outcomes of interest were measured during the TSST- examinationsessions.Physiologicalaswellaspsychometric measureswereevaluated.Stressreactivityofrepeatedsali- varycortisollevels(i.e.theinteractiongroup-by-stress)was definedasthemainoutcomemeasure.Secondarymeasures includedrepeateda-amylase,heartrate,anddifferentpsy- chometricassessmenttools.

2.6. Physiologicalmeasures

Salivasamples (Salivette1;Sarstedt AG,Sevelen,Switzer- land) were obtainedfor determination of salivary cortisol (10min( 20min)and1min( 10min)priortotheTSSTand

Table1 Demographic,Taiji-related,andpsychometricgroupcharacteristicsofthe49subjectsunderstudywhocompletedthe TrierSocialStressTest(TSST).

Groupcharacteristics Taijigroup(n=26) Controlgroup(n=23) p

Agea(years) 35.771.61 35.741.31 .99

Gender(male/female) 8/18 9/14 .56

MenstrualcyclephaseatTSSTexaminationday (femalesubjectsinlutealphase/infollicularphase)

9/9 4/10 .29

Bodymassindex(kg/m2) 23.47.67 23.51.79 .97

Education(with/withouthighschooldegreei.e.Swiss‘‘Matura’’) 20/6 17/6 1.00

Occupationalstatus(fullorparttimeworkers/students) 24/2 23/0 .49

Smoking(nonsmokers/lightsmokersb) 21/5 18/5 1.00

Sportiveactivity—duringtheintervention(h/week) 2.40.37 2.98.52 .37

Previousexperiencewithmind—bodypractices (monthsofregularpractice;preintervention)

15.626.03 29.1310.68 .26

Depressiveaffect(ADS-Kscore) 10.881.35 11.041.45 .94

Perceivedstress(PSSscore) 17.461.03 17.911.22 .78

Taijiclassesattended(incl.%-value) 20.65.59(86%) — —

Taijipracticeathome(numberofsessions/week) 1.69.33 — —

aContinuousdataareexpressedasmeanS.E.M.

b Smokinglessthan5cigarettesperday.

(5)

immediately(+1min)aswellas10,20,30,45and60minafter stress cessation) anda-amylase levels ( 20min, 10min, +1min,+10min,+20min,+45min).Sampleswerestoredat 208C until assaying. After thawing, saliva samples were prepared for biochemical analysis by centrifuging at 3000rpm for 5min to produce a clear supernatant of low viscosity.Estimationofsalivaryfree cortisolwasperformed usingachemiluminescenceimmunoassaywithhighsensitivity of0.16ng/ml(IBLHamburg,Germany).Levelsofa-amylase were determined following previously described methods (RohlederandNater,2009).Bothsalivarycortisolandsalivary a-amylasewereanalyzedinacommerciallaboratory(Dresden LabServiceGmbH,Dresden,Germany).Inter-andintra-assay coefficientsofvariationwerebothbelow8%(cortisol),and10%

(a-amylase),respectively.Asingle-channelelectrocardiogram (ECG,standardlead) wasrecordedcontinuouslyat 4036Hz throughouttheexperimentalsessionusing aportableheart rate(HR)monitoringdevice(MedikorderMK3,TOM-Medical, Graz,Austria).HRdatawasaggregatedto5minHRsegments.

Thefirst5minHRsegment( 10min)wasdefinedasbaseline.

HR segments measured before ( 5min), during (+5min, +10min)and after thestress task (+15min,+20min)were consideredinstatisticalanalyses.

2.7. Psychometricmeasures

Baseline group characteristics included assessment of per- ceived stress and depression symptoms. Perceived stress wasassessedbytheGermanversionofthePerceivedStress Scale(PSS)(CohenandWilliamson,1988).This10-itemself- report questionnaire measures subjects’ evaluation of the stressfulnessofthesituationsexperiencedinthepastmonth oftheirlives.ItemsinthePSSweredesignedtoassesshow predictable,uncontrollableandoverloadingparticipantscon- sidertheirlives.Goodinternalconsistencyisreported(Cron- bach’s a=.78). Depressive affect and negative thought patternswere measured by usingthe ‘‘Allgemeine Depres- sionsskala-Kurzform’’(ADS-K)questionnaire(Hautzingerand Bailer,1993),theGermanversionofthe‘‘CenterforEpide- miologicalStudiesDepressionScale’’(CES-D)(Radloff,1977).

Thisquestionnairewasdevelopedforresearchinthegeneral populationand has showngoodinternal consistency (Cron- bach’sa=.90).WemeasuredpsychologicalTSSTstressreac- tivityatbaselineandimmediatelyafterstresscessation:the MultidimensionalMood Questionnaire(MDMQ) assessesself- reportedmoodandcalmnesswithgoodinternalconsistencies (‘‘mood’’—Cronbach’sa=.75—.87;‘‘calmness’’—Cronbach’s a=.77—.83)(Steyeretal.,1997).Psychologicalevaluationof perceived stressfulness during the TSST examination was obtainedbycompletionofavisualanaloguescale(VAS)ranging from0to10with0indicatingnostressexperiencedatall.

2.8. Statisticalanalysis

Datawereanalyzedusing SPSS(version 18)statisticalsoft- warepackageforMacintosh(IBMSPSSStatistics.Somers,NY, USA).Thecalculationof theoptimaltotalsamplesizehas been conducted using the statistical software G-Power (Buchneretal.,1997).Basedonpriorresearch oncortisol stressresponsesreportingeffectsizesrangingfromf2=.28—

.35(Gaabetal.,2003;Storchetal.,2007),theoptimaltotal

sample sizeof N=64was calculated a priori to detect an expectedmediumtolargeeffectsizeoff2=.25withapower .85 anda=.05 (effect size conventions:f2: .02=small, .15=medium, .35=large; see Cohen, 1988). Effect size parameters (f)werecalculatedfrom partialh2-valuesand arereportedwhereappropriate(effectsizeconventions:f: .10=small,.25=medium,.40=large;seeCohen,1988).All analysesweretwo-tailed,withthelevelofsignificancesetat p<.05andthelevelofborderlinesignificanceat p<.10.

Unlessindicated,allresultsarepresentedasmeanstan- standarderrorofmeans(S.E.M.).Priortostatisticalanalyses alldataweretestedfornormaldistributionandhomogeneity ofvarianceusingaKolmogorov—SmirnovandLevenetest.As cortisol levels were skewed we log-transformed (log10) cortisoldataandobtainedanormaldistribution.Log-trans- formedcortisoldatawereusedinstatisticalanalysesbutfor reasonsofclarityuntransformeddataaredepictedinFig.2a.

Group characteristics were analyzed by x2-analysis for categorical data, andindependent samples t-test for con- tinuous data. Group differences in TSST related baseline valueswerealsotestedbyt-tests.

Torevealpossibletimeandconditioneffects,repeatedly measured physiological and psychological data were ana- lyzed by using two way ANCOVAs with repeated measure- mentswithgroupastheindependentfactor(Taijigroupvs.

controlgroup)andcortisol,heartrate,a-amylase,perceived stressfulness,mood,andcalmnessdataasrepeateddepen- dent factors. We applied Huynh—Feldt correction where appropriate.

Topreventovercontrollinggivenoursamplesize(Babyak, 2004),we performedatwo-stepprocedureforanalysesof physiologicalparameters.Inthefirststep,representingthe mainanalysisfortheprimaryoutcomemeasurecortisol,we calculated repeatedcortisol ANCOVAswhilecontrolling for cortisolbaselinelevels,priorexperiencewithself-applicable mind—bodypractices,age,menstrualcyclephase,andgen- der asa priori definedcovariates. Significanteffectswere furthertestedinasecondstep,whereweadditionallycon- trolled for smoking and BMI, as well as regular physical activityduringtheinterventionperiod.Analysesfora-amy- laseandheartratewerecalculatedaccordingly.Inanalyses of repeated psychological data, we controlled for prior mind—bodypracticeexperienceasacovariate.

Posthoctestingofsignificanteffectsinthemainanalyses includedseparaterecalculationofthepreviouslydescribed ANCOVAanalysesforeachoftherepeatedtimepoints.

3. Results

Ofthe112subjectswhounderwentatelephonescreening, 40 subjects did not fulfil selection criteria. Reasons for exclusion anddrop-out aredocumented in Fig.1. Of the remaining74subjects,70successfullyunderwentbaseline examinationandwererandomlyassignedtoeithertheTaiji group (N=35)or thewaitingcontrolgroup (N=35).TSST examination wascompletedby26subjects fromthe Taiji group(meanage35.771.61;69%female)andby23sub- jectsfromthecontrol group(meanage 35.741.31; 61%

female)as21subjectsdroppedoutbeforetheTSSTordidnot fulfilinclusioncriteriafortheTSST(Fig.1).Since95%ofall dropouts have not attended the TSST examination, an

(6)

intention-to-treatapproachisnotapplicabletothisstudy design.Thereforeonly subjectscompletingtheTSSTwere includedindataanalysis.Wehadnomissingdata.Noadverse effectsoftheTaijitrainingwereobservedorreported.

3.1. Groupcharacteristics

The two study groups did notsignificantly differ in group characteristic(Table1)anddrop-outrate(p=.603).Drop- out subjects did not significantly differ from the subjects completing the study in any group characteristic (p’s>.415),except BMI(21.17.49 (drop-outgroup) vs.

23.49.51(finalstudygroup);p<.001).

3.2. Physiologicalstressreactivity

At baseline,the studygroups didnotdiffer incortisol, a- amylase, or heart rate. The TSST induced significant increases in all physiological measures under study (main effectsofstress:p’s<.001).Whencontrollingforconfoun- ders considered in the main analyses, a significant main effect of stress was observed for cortisol (p<.001) and heartrate(p=.027),butnotfora-amylase(p=.91).

3.2.1. Cortisol

TheTaijigroupshowedanattenuatedcortisolstressreactiv- ityascomparedtothecontrolgroupwhilecontrollingforthe firstsetofconfounders(i.e.physiologicalbaselinelevel,age, gender,menstrualcyclephase,andpriormind—bodyprac- tice experience) [interaction group-by-stress: F(2.92/

122.50)=3.16,p=.028,partialh2=.07,f=.27;maineffect of group: F(1/0.79)=2.99, p=0.091; Fig. 2a]. Additional controllingforthesecondsetofconfounders(i.e.smoking status,BMI,andphysicalactivity)didnotsignificantlychange results(p=.044,resp. p=.122).Posthoctestsrevealeda trendtowardslowercortisollevels10min[F(1/0.22)=3.80, p=.058],30min [F(1/0.20)=2.96,p=.093],45min[F(1/

0.20)=3.71,p=.061]and60minafterstresscessation[F(1/

0.20)=3.80,p=.058]intheTaijigroup,suggestingalower increase and a faster recovery of salivary cortisol in the interventiongroup(seeFig.2a).

3.2.2. a-Amylase

Comparedtocontrols,participantsoftheTaijigroupshowed significantlower a-amylaseactivitybeforeandafterstress whilecontrollingforthefirstsetofconfounders[maineffect ofgroup:F(1/100795.10)=4.12,p=.049,partialh2=.089, f=.31;Fig.2b].Nosignificantgroupdifferencewasfoundfor a-amylase stress reactivity [interaction group-by-stress:

p=.16]. After additional consideration of the second set ofconfoundersthemaineffectofgroupremainedsignificant (p=.040) and a trend towards reduced a-amylase stress reactivity in participants of the Taiji group was revealed [interaction group-by-stress: p=.086]. Post hoc testing showed significantly lower a-amylase levels 10min [F(1/

40874.38)=6.63, p=.014], 20min [F(1/23952.02)=4.03, p=.051] and 45min after stress cessation [F(1/

41612.24)=8.66, p=.005] in the Taijigroup as compared to the control group, indicating a faster recovery (see Fig.2b).

Figure2 Valuesare meansS.E.M.WecalculatedANCOVAs withrepeated measuresofphysiological stress parametersas dependentvariables andgroup(Taijivs.control)astheinde- pendentvariable.Wecontrolledforphysiologicalbaselinelevel, age,gender,menstrualcyclephaseand priorexperiencewith mind—body practices as covariates. The Taiji group showed attenuated cortisol stress reactivity (p=.028; a), a-amylase levels(p=.049;b),aswellaslowerheartratestressresponses (p=.028; c). Significance levels are: 8p<.1; *p<.05;

**p<.01.

(7)

3.2.3. Heartrate

Themainanalysisrevealedasignificantlybluntedheartrate stressreactivity intheTaijigroupcomparedtothecontrol group [interaction group-by-stress: F(2.55/173.76)=3.34, p=.028, partial h2=.087, f=.31; Fig. 2c]. Furthermore, theparticipantsoftheTaijigroupshowed atrendtowards lowerheartratelevelsbeforeandafterthestressprotocol [maineffectofgroup:F(1/750.01)=3.15,p=.083;Fig.2c].

Additionalcontrollingforthesecondsetofconfoundersdid notsignificantlychangeresults.Posthoctestsrevealedthat participantsoftheTaijigroupexhibited significantlylower heartratelevelsduringthefirst5min[F(1/559.40)=5.93, p=.019], and the second 5min [F(1/569.63)=4.33, p=.044]oftheTSSTascomparedtocontrols(seeFig.2c).

3.3. Psychologicalstressreactivity

Atbaselinesubjectivemeasuresofperceivedstressfulness, mood,andcalmnessdidnotdifferbetweenstudygroups.The TSST significantly increased perceived stressfulness, wor- senedmood,andreducedcalmnessinallstudyparticipants (main effects of stress: p’s<.001). Controlling for prior experience with self-applicable mind—body practices did notsignificantlychangefindings.

Comparedtothecontrols,participantsintheTaijigroup reported significantly less stressfulness [F(1/16.37)=8.48, p=.006,partialh2=.156,f=.43],maintainedahigherlevel of calmness [F(1/21.79)=5.87, p=.019, partial h2=.113, effectsizef=.36]andtendedtowardsalowerdecreaseof mood[F(1/17.78)=3.43,p=.070]in reactionto theTSST (seeTable2).

4. Discussion

This is the first randomized-controlled study to explore effectsofTaijion measuresof adrenocortical,autonomic, andpsychologicalresponsestoastandardizedandvalidated psychosocialstresstaskinhealthyTaijibeginners.Wefound forthefirst timemarkedlyreduced psychobiologicalstress responses in Taijipractitioners as compared to anon-Taiji controlgroup,i.e.attenuatedcortisolandheartratestress reactivity,lowera-amylaselevels,aswellaslowerperceived stressfulnessandbettermaintenanceofcalmnessinresponse to the stress task. Baseline values did notdiffer between

groups and stress induction proved to be successful, as indicatedbytheexpectedsignificantincreasesinallphysio- logicalmeasuresunderstudyinthetotalsample.

Thepresentresultsextendpreviousresearchbysuggest- ingan overallstress-buffering effect ofTaijipracticeon a broad array of measures representing different stress- responsive systemswith effect sizes rangingfrom medium tolarge.Notably,werecruitedTaijibeginnersandourTaiji intervention lasted for 12weeks. As Taiji is thought to improveitsbeneficialeffectswithincreasingpracticeskills over years (Cheng, 1982), it can be speculated that the observedeffectsmaybeevenmorepronouncedinadvanced Taijipractitioners.

In contrast to stress management interventions (Gaab et al., 2003; Hammerfald et al., 2006; Storch et al., 2007), the Taiji course in our study was neither designed nortaughtasaformofstressmanagement.Itwasconcep- tualizedtoconveyanembodimentofbasicTaijiprinciplesby applying aguidedintrospectiveteachingapproach.Wedid nottrainanyspecificcopingstrategy(e.g.cognitiverestruc- turing) nor did we use role-plays or psychodrama course elements as often used in cognitive behavioural (Gaab etal.,2003;Hammerfaldetal.,2006)andresourceactivat- ingstressmanagementprogrammes(Storchetal.,2007).In contrasttoourTaijicourse,trainingelementsofsuchstress managementprogrammes may,inadditiontotheirspecific effectsonstressappraisal,havemoresimilaritiestotheTSST situation andtherefore might additionally preparefor the stresstestitself.Consideringthelackingemphasisonstress management in the Taiji intervention, the incongruence between the training environment and the TSST setting, and the focus on developing Taijirelated bodyawareness and body mechanics, we feel that the stress protective effectsofTaijiobservedinourstudyarelikelytoresultfrom amindfulembodimentofeffortlessstabilityandcalmnessin motion. Thisreasoning is further supported byour finding that,similarlytotheTaijieffect,thecontrolvariable‘‘prior experiencewithmind—bodypractices’’(otherthanTaiji)was significantlyassociatedwithbluntedcortisolstressreactiv- ity, lower a-amylase levels, as well as lower perceived stressfulness andbettermaintenance of moodin response tothestresstask(p’s<.040).

Priorresearchfurthersupportsthattheobservedattenua- tion of psychobiologicalstressreactivity in ourTaijigroup mayrelatetomind—bodyinteractioneffects.Anincreased Table2 PsychologicalreactivityinresponsetotheTrierSocialStressTest(TSST).a

Variables Taijigroup(n=26) Controlgroup(n=23) pc

PreTSST PostTSST Stresschangeb PreTSST PostTSST Stresschangeb Self-reported

stressfulness(VAS)d

1.22.19 3.47.45 2.25.39 1.01.20 4.91.48 3.90.41 .006 Calmness(MDMS)e 16.47.46 12.78.62 -3.69.54 16.25.49 10.64.66 5.61.57 .019 Mood(MDMS)e 17.30.44 14.23.74 -3.07.64 17.19.47 12.39.79 -4.80.68 .070

aAlldataareexpressedasmeanS.E.M.

bStresschange postTSSTvalueminuspreTSSTvalue.

c p-ValuesrefertorepeatedmeasuresANCOVAswithpriormind bodypracticeasacovariate.

dVAS visualanaloguescalerangingfrom0 ‘notstressfulatall’to10 ‘extremelystressful’.

eMDMS multidimensionalmoodscalerangingfrom5to20withhigherscoresindicatingahigherdegreeofcalmnessandamorepositive mood.

(8)

bodyawarenessinducedbyregularTaijipracticehasbeen reportedinpreviousstudies(Gyllenstenetal.,2010;Uhlig etal.,2010)andislikelyto enhancearesource activating embodiment.Maintainingresourceactivatingembodimentin turnhasbeenshowntoreducecortisollevelsunderresting conditions(Carneyetal.,2010).Moreover,copingstrategies includingembodimentwereanintegratedpartofaresource activatingstressmanagement programmefoundto attenu- atethereactivity of theHPA-axis inresponse to the TSST (Storchetal.,2007).AsparticipantsdidnotreportanyTaiji- participationinducedincrease insocialcontacts (datanot shown),itseemsunlikelythattheobservedstress-buffering intheTaijigrouprelates to atraining-induced increasein socialsupport.However,itmaybespeculatedthatsubjects intheTaijigroup,becauseoftheirparticipationintheactive studygroup,mighthaveexpectedtobebetterpreparedfor theirupcomingperformancetaskandthusachievedgreater emotionregulationduringtheTSST.Toclarifythepotential contributionofsuchanexpectancyeffect,futureresearchis needed,preferablybyincludinganadditionalactivecontrol groupwithaninterventionraisingsimilarexpectations.

Our studyhas severalstrengths. First, we used a well- validatedstandardizedacutepsychosocialstresstask(Kirsch- baumetal.,1993;DickersonandKemeny,2004).Second,we usedanon-Taijicontrolgroupwithrandomizedassignment.

Third,weassessedmultipleparametersindicatingreactivity ofdifferentindependentstressresponsivesystems.Fourth, baselinecharacteristicswerethoroughlycollectedandboth studygroupswerehomogenousregardingtheirdemographic andpsychometricparameters,indicatingasuccessfulrando- mization of subjects. Finally, both groups had moderate scores in questionnaires assessing baseline levels of per- ceivedstressanddepressiveaffect.Itthereforeseemsunli- kelythatthereportedresultsareinfluencedbypre-existing group differences or selection bias related to increased pronenesstostress.

Thefollowinglimitationsneedtobeconsidered.First,our resultsarerestrictedtoagroupofhealthyandwell-educated young to middle-aged men and women. They cannot be generalizedto other groupswith lessadvantageous health conditionsorsocialbackgrounds.Second,theretrospectively assessedaveragenumberofTaijihomepracticesessionsper week,theaveragetimespentonsportiveactivitiesperweek duringtheinterventionperiod,andthedeterminationofthe menstrualcyclephasewerebasedonself-report.Third,our resultsare restrictedto Taijinovices.The effectsoflong- termTaijipracticeonpsychobiologicalstress-reactivitystill needtobeinvestigated.Fourth,ourpsychobiologicalassess- mentapproachdoesnotincludeassessmentoffurtherstress- responsivephysiological systems such as theimmune, the lipid, or the coagulation system. Also, HPA axis measures otherthancortisolsuchascorticotropinreleasinghormone (CRH),oradrenocorticotrophichormone(ACTH)stillneedto be examined. Fifth, because of habituation of cortisol responses in the majority of people repeating the TSST (Schommeretal.,2003),itwasnotpossibletoassesscortisol stressresponsesbeforeandaftertheintervention,norinthe controlgroupaftercompletionoftheirTaijicourse.Sixth, ournon-significanteffectsofTaijiona-amylasestressreac- tivityshouldbeinterpretedwithcareaswecannotruleouta typeIIerror.Futurestudies,preferablywithahigherpower, areneededtoconfirmournon-significanteffectsofTaijion

a-amylase stress reactivity as well as the overall stress- reducingeffectsontheothermeasuresunderstudy.Finally, duetoourrestrictiveexclusioncriteriawe hada drop-out rateof30%.However,thisrateiscomparabletoapriorTSST studyexaminingmind-bodypractices(Paceetal.,2009)and doesnotseemunusualinstudiesexaminingTaijieffectson psychologicalwell-being(Wangetal.,2010a).

Inconclusion,ourresultsconsistentlysuggestthatpracti- cingTaijiattenuatespsychobiologicalstressreactivity.This mayunderlinetheroleofTaijiasausefulmind—bodyprac- ticeforstresspreventionwhichmaycontributetoenhance healthinthegeneralpopulation.Clinicalimplicationsremain tobeelucidated.

Role of funding source

FundingforthisstudywasprovidedbyStiftungfu¨rKomple- menta¨rmedizin,GottfriedundJuliaBangerter-RhynerStiftung and Parrotia Stiftung (to M.N.) and by the Swiss National FoundationGrantPP00P1 128565/1(toP.H.W.).Thefunding sourceshadnofurtherroleinstudydesign;inthecollection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report;andinthedecisiontosubmitthepaperforpublication.

Conflict of interest

Allauthorsdeclarethattheyhavenoconflictsofinterest.

Acknowledgements

We thankBarbara Schwab, Christina Bu¨rgler, Tina Camen- zind,Marina Haller andNikola Nedeljkovic for their assis- tance in conducting the study, and Dr. Ursula Wolf and Dietrich von Bonin for their technical support andhelpful advises.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary dataassociated with thisarticle can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.psy- neuen.2011.12.007.

References

Babyak,M.A.,2004.Whatyouseemaynotbewhatyouget:abrief, nontechnicalintroductiontooverfittinginregression-typemod- els.Psychosom.Med.66,411 421.

Benson,S.,Arck,P.C.,Tan,S.,Mann,K.,Hahn,S.,Janssen,O.E., Schedlowski, M., Elsenbruch, S., 2009. Effects of obesity on neuroendocrine,cardiovascular,and immunecell responsesto acutepsychosocialstressinpremenopausalwomen.Psychoneur- oendocrinology34,181 189.

Brotman,D.J.,Golden,S.H.,Wittstein,I.S.,2007.Thecardiovascu- lartollofstress.Lancet370,1089 1100.

Buchner,A.,Faul,F.,Erdfelder,E.,1997.G.Power:APriori,PostHoc, andCompromisePowerAnalysesfortheMacintosh(Version2.1.2) UniversityofTrier,Trier,Germany.

Carney, D.R., Cuddy, A.J., Yap, A.J., 2010. Power posing: brief nonverbaldisplaysaffectneuroendocrinelevelsandrisktoler- ance.Psychol.Sci.21,1363 1368.

(9)

Cheng, M., 1982. Master Cheng’s Thirteen Chapters on T’ai Chi Chuan.SweetChiPress,NewYork.

Chida,Y.,Hamer,M.,2008.Chronicpsychosocialfactorsandacute physiologicalresponsestolaboratory-inducedstressinhealthy populations:aquantitativereviewof30yearsofinvestigations.

Psychol.Bull.134,829 885.

Chida,Y.,Steptoe,A.,2010. Greatercardiovascularresponsesto laboratorymental stress are associatedwith poor subsequent cardiovascular riskstatus:a meta-analysis ofprospectiveevi- dence.Hypertension55,1026 1032.

Cohen,J.,1988.StatisticalPowerAnalysisfortheBehaviorSciences.

LawrenceEarlbaumAssociates,Hillsdale,NJ.

Cohen,S.,Williamson,G.,1988.Perceivedstressinaprobability sample of the United States. In: Spacapan, S., Oskamp, S.

(Eds.),TheSocialPsychologyofHealth:ClaremontSymposium on Applied Social Psychology. Sage Publications, Newsbury Park,CA, pp.31 67.

Dickerson, S.S.,Kemeny, M.E.,2004. Acute stressorsandcortisol responses:atheoreticalintegrationandsynthesisoflaboratory research.Psychol.Bull.130,355 391.

Ehlert,U.,Gaab,J.,Heinrichs,M.,2001.Psychoneuroendocrinolo- gicalcontributionstotheetiologyofdepression,posttraumatic stressdisorder,andstress-relatedbodilydisorders:theroleofthe hypothalamus pituitary adrenal axis.Biol. Psychol.57,141 152.

Esch, T., Duckstein, J., Welke, J., Braun, V., 2007. Mind/body techniquesforphysiologicalandpsychologicalstressreduction:

stressmanagementviaTaiChitraining apilotstudy.Med.Sci.

Monit.13,CR488 CR497.

Gaab,J.,Blattler,N.,Menzi,T.,Pabst,B.,Stoyer,S., Ehlert,U., 2003.Randomizedcontrolledevaluationoftheeffectsofcogni- tive-behavioralstressmanagementoncortisolresponsestoacute stressinhealthysubjects.Psychoneuroendocrinology28,767 779.

Gyllensten,A.L.,Hui-Chan,C.W.,Tsang,W.W.,2010.Stabilitylimits, single-leg jump,and body awarenessin older Tai Chi practi- tioners.Arch.Phys.Med.Rehabil.91,215 220.

Hamer, M., O’Donnell, K.,Lahiri, A.,Steptoe, A.,2010. Salivary cortisolresponsestomentalstressareassociatedwithcoronary arterycalcificationinhealthymenandwomen.Eur.HeartJ.31, 424 429.

Hammerfald,K.,Eberle,C.,Grau,M.,Kinsperger,A.,Zimmermann, A.,Ehlert,U.,Gaab, J.,2006.Persistenteffectsofcognitive- behavioral stress management on cortisol responses to acute stressinhealthysubjects arandomizedcontrolledtrial.Psy- choneuroendocrinology31,333 339.

Hautzinger,M.,Bailer,M.,1993.AllgemeineDepressions-Skala.Center forEpidemiologicalStudiesDepressionScale,Beltz,Weinheim.

Jahnke, R., Larkey, L., Rogers, C., Etnier, J., Lin, F., 2010. A comprehensivereviewofhealthbenefitsofQigongandTaiChi.

Am.J.HealthPromot.24,e1 e25.

Jin,P.,1989.Changesinheartrate,noradrenaline,cortisolandmood duringTaiChi.J.Psychosom.Res.33,197 206.

Jin,P., 1992. Efficacy ofTai Chi, brisk walking,meditation, and readinginreducingmentalandemotionalstress.J.Psychosom.

Res.36,361 370.

Kirschbaum, C.,Pirke, K.M., Hellhammer, D.H.,1993. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28,76 81.

Kirschbaum,C.,Kudielka,B.M.,Gaab,J.,Schommer,N.C.,Hellham- mer,D.H.,1999.Impactofgender,menstrualcyclephase,and oralcontraceptivesontheactivityofthehypothalamus pitui- tary adrenalaxis.Psychosom.Med.61,154 162.

Klein,P.J.,Adams,W.D.,2004.Comprehensivetherapeuticbenefits ofTaiji:acriticalreview.Am.J.Phys.Med.Rehabil.83,735 745.

Kudielka, B.M., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirsch- baum,C.,2004.HPAaxisresponsestolaboratorypsychosocial

stressin healthyelderlyadults,youngeradults,andchildren:

impactofageandgender.Psychoneuroendocrinology29,83 98.

Lenton,E.A.,Landgren,B.M.,Sexton,L.,1984.Normalvariationinthe lengthofthelutealphaseofthemenstrualcycle:identificationof theshortlutealphase.Br.J.Obstet.Gynaecol.91,685 689.

Lovallo,W.R.,Gerin,W.,2003.Psychophysiologicalreactivity:mech- anismsandpathwaystocardiovasculardisease.Psychosom.Med.

65,36 45.

Pace,T.W.,Negi,L.T.,Adame,D.D.,Cole,S.P.,Sivilli,T.I.,Brown, T.D.,Issa,M.J.,Raison,C.L.,2009.Effectofcompassionmedita- tiononneuroendocrine,innateimmuneandbehavioralresponses topsychosocialstress.Psychoneuroendocrinology34,87 98.

Radloff,L.S.,1977.TheCES-DScale:aself-reportdepressionscale forresearchinthegeneralpopulation.Appl.Psychol.Meas.1, 385 401.

Raison,C.L.,Miller,A.H.,2003.Whennotenoughistoomuch:the roleofinsufficientglucocorticoidsignalinginthepathophysiology ofstress-relateddisorders.Am.J.Psychiatry160,1554 1565.

Rimmele,U.,Zellweger,B.C.,Marti,B.,Seiler,R.,Mohiyeddini,C., Ehlert,U.,Heinrichs,M.,2007.Trainedmenshowlowercortisol, heartrateandpsychologicalresponsestopsychosocialstresscom- paredwithuntrainedmen.Psychoneuroendocrinology32,627 635.

Robins,J.L.,McCain,N.L.,Gray,D.P.,Elswick,R.K.,Walter,J.M., McDade,E.,2006.Researchonpsychoneuroimmunology:TaiChi asastressmanagementapproachforindividualswithHIVdisease.

Appl.Nurs.Res.19,2 9.

Robinson,R.,2006.TaiChiforYou TheComprehensiveGuidetoTai ChiatHomeforEverybody.DuncanBraidPublishers,London.

Rohleder, N.,Kirschbaum,C.,2006.Thehypothalamic pituitary adrenal(HPA)axisinhabitualsmokers.Int.J.Psychophysiol.59, 236 243.

Rohleder, N., Nater, U.M., 2009. Determinants ofsalivary alpha- amylaseinhumansandmethodologicalconsiderations.Psycho- neuroendocrinology34,469 485.

Rosmond,R.,Bjorntorp,P.,2000.Thehypothalamic pituitary ad- renalaxisactivityasapredictorofcardiovasculardisease,type2 diabetesandstroke.J.Intern.Med.247,188 197.

Rozanski,A.,Blumenthal,J.A.,Kaplan,J.,1999.Impactofpsycho- logicalfactorsonthepathogenesisofcardiovasculardiseaseand implicationsfortherapy.Circulation99,2192 2217.

Rozanski, A., Blumenthal,J.A.,Davidson, K.W.,Saab, P.G., Kub- zansky,L.,2005.Theepidemiology,pathophysiology,andman- agement of psychosocial risk factors incardiac practice: the emerging field of behavioral cardiology. J.Am. Coll. Cardiol.

45,637 651.

Schommer,N.C.,Hellhammer,D.H.,Kirschbaum,C.,2003.Dissocia- tionbetweenreactivityofthehypothalamus pituitary adrenal axisandthesympathetic-adrenal-medullarysystemtorepeated psychosocialstress.Psychosom.Med.65,450 460.

Shonkoff, J.P., Boyce, W.T., McEwen, B.S., 2009. Neuroscience, molecularbiology,andthechildhoodrootsofhealthdisparities:

building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention.JAMA301,2252 2259.

Steptoe, A.,Donald,A.E., O’Donnell,K.,Marmot,M., Deanfield, J.E.,2006.Delayedbloodpressurerecoveryafterpsychological stressisassociatedwithcarotidintima-mediathickness:White- hallpsychobiologystudy. Arterioscler.Thromb. Vasc.Biol. 26, 2547 2551.

Steptoe, A., Hamer, M., Chida, Y., 2007. The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humans: areviewandmeta-analysis.Brain Behav.Immun.21, 901 912.

Steyer,R.,Schwenkmezger,P.,Notz,P.,Eid,M.,1997.DerMehrdi- mensionaleBefindlichkeitsfragebogen(MDBF).(MDMQ Multidi- mensionalMoodQuestionnaire).Hogrefe,Go¨ttingen.

Storch,M.,Gaab,J.,Kuttel,Y.,Stussi,A.C.,Fend,H.,2007.Psy- choneuroendocrineeffectsofresource-activatingstressmanage- menttraining.HealthPsychol.26,456 463.

(10)

Taylor-Piliae,R.E.,Haskell,W.L.,Waters, C.M.,Froelicher,E.S., 2006. Change in perceived psychosocial status following a 12-week Tai Chi exercise programme. J. Adv. Nurs. 54, 313 329.

U.S. National Institutes of Health, 2010. http://nccam.nih.gov/

health/whatiscam/.

Uhlig,T.,Fongen, C.,Steen,E., Christie,A.,Odegard,S., 2010.

Exploring Tai Chi in rheumatoid arthritis: a quantitative and qualitativestudy.BMCMusculoskelet.Disord.11,43.

vonKanel,R.,Mills,P.J.,Fainman,C.,Dimsdale,J.E.,2001.Effects ofpsychologicalstressandpsychiatricdisordersonbloodcoagu- lationandfibrinolysis:abiobehavioralpathwaytocoronaryartery disease? Psychosom.Med.63,531 544.

Wang,C.,Bannuru,R.,Ramel,J.,Kupelnick,B.,Scott,T.,Schmid, C.H., 2010a.Tai Chi on psychologicalwell-being: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Complement. Altern. Med.

10,23.

Wang,C.,Schmid,C.H.,Rones,R.,Kalish,R.,Yinh,J.,Goldenberg, D.L.,Lee,Y.,McAlindon,T.,2010b.Arandomizedtrialoftaichi forfibromyalgia.N.Engl.J.Med.363,743 754.

Wayne,P.M.,Kaptchuk,T.J.,2008.ChallengesinherenttoT’aiChi research:partI T’aiChiasacomplexmulticomponentinter- vention.J.Altern.Complement.Med.14,95 102.

Wolf,S.L.,Coogler,C.,Xu,T.,1997.ExploringthebasisforTaiChi Chuan as a therapeutic exercise approach. Arch. Phys. Med.

Rehabil.78,886 892.

Yeh,G.Y., Wood, M.J., Lorell, H.B., Stevenson,L.W., Eisenberg, D.M.,Wayne,P.M.,Goldberger,A.L.,Davis,R.B.,Phillips,R.S., 2004.EffectsofTaiChimind bodymovementtherapyonfunc- tionalstatusandexercisecapacityinpatientswithchronicheart failure:arandomizedcontrolledtrial.Am.J.Med.117,541 548.

Yeh,G.Y.,McCarthy,E.P.,Wayne,P.M.,Stevenson,L.W.,Wood,M.J., Forman,D., Davis,R.B.,Phillips,R.S.,2011.TaiChiexercise in patientswithchronicheartfailure.Arch.Intern.Med.171,750 757.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

and significance in both Mann–Whitney U and Wilcoxon Matched Pairs tests confirmed what has been found from exercise median normalized results (outcome passiveROM / outcome

Effect of the size of zona pellucida opening by laser assisted hatching on clinical outcome of frozen cleaved embryos that were cultured to blastocyst after thawing in women with

Resting state brain network analysis in Beta showed that inter-hemispheric connectivity (expressed as number of connections) was significantly lower in the overall

Musculoskeletal pain is common and so far no experiment tested the acute effects of a single stochastic resonance whole-body vibration training (SR-WBV) on muscle relaxation and

In this RCT, we investigated the effects of the tablet- based program in adults with low-grade glioma and menin- gioma, cognitive performance as primary outcome and self-

The main goal of this work was to determine the impact of standardized post-coercion review sessions on the experi- ence of peritraumatic distress and posttraumatic symptoms

First, a generalized linear mixed-effects model assuming a binomial error distribution was used to analyze whether there were differences in the proportions of participants

The GAMIN study was an individually randomized 1:1 trial comparing a single oral dose of azithromycin (20  mg/kg) or matching placebo to better understand potential mechanisms