• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Reprint of "Affective picture processing as a function of preceding picture valence : an ERP analysis"

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Reprint of "Affective picture processing as a function of preceding picture valence : an ERP analysis""

Copied!
6
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Reprint of “Affective picture processing as a function of preceding picture valence: An ERP analysis”

Harald T. Schupp

a,∗

, Ralf Schmälzle

a

, Tobias Flaisch

a

, Almut I. Weike

b

, Alfons O. Hamm

b

aDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofKonstanz,Germany

bDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofGreifswald,Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o

Keywords:

Affect Attention Pictures

Event-relatedpotentials Earlyposteriornegativity Latepositivepotential

a b s t r a c t

Event-relatedbrainpotential(ERP)studiesconsistentlyrevealedthatarelativelyearly(earlyposterior negativity;EPN)andalate(latepositivepotential;LPP)ERPcomponentdifferentiatebetweenemotional andneutralpicturestimuli.Twostudiesexaminedtheprocessingofemotionalstimuliwhenpreceded eitherbypleasant,neutral,orunpleasantcontextimages.Inbothstudies,distinctstreamsofsixpictures wereshown.InStudy1,hedoniccontextwasalternatedrandomlyacrossthe180picturestreams.InStudy 2,hedoniccontextsequenceswereblocked,resultingin60precedingsequencesofpleasant,neutral,and unpleasantcontextvalence,respectively.Themainfindingwasthatthevalenceoftheprecedingpicture sequencehadnosignificanteffectontheemotionalmodulationoftheEPNandLPPcomponents.However, previousresultswerereplicatedinthatemotionalstimulusprocessingwasassociatedwithlargerEPNand LPPcomponentsascomparedtoneutralpictures.Thesefindingssuggestthattheprioritizedprocessing ofemotionalstimuliisprimarilydrivenbythevalenceofthecurrentpicture.

1. Introduction

In a world where various stimuli compete for attentional resources,thefastandreliabledetectionofpositiveandnegative reinforcersfacilitatesadaptivebehavior(Langetal.,1997;Öhman etal.,2000).Thus,functionalandevolutionaryconsiderationssug- gestthepreferentialprocessingofemotionalcues.Overthepast decade,numerousneuroimagingstudieshaveconfirmedthatemo- tionalcuesguideselectivevisualattention.Functionalmagnetic resonanceimaging(fMRI)revealedincreasedBOLD(bloodoxygen level dependent)signalsin associative visualregions (extrastri- ate,occipito-parietal,andinferiortemporalcortex)andsubcortical limbic structures when viewing emotionally arousing pictures comparedtoneutralpictures(e.g.,Bradleyetal.,2003;Costafreda etal.,2008;Junghöferetal.,2006;Sabatinellietal.,2007,2011).

Furthermore,event-relatedbrainpotential(ERP)studiesrevealed thetemporaldynamicsofemotionprocessingatthelevelofdistinct processingstagesanddemonstratedthatemotionalstimulireceive enhancedprocessingearlyintheprocessingstream(Schuppetal., 2006).

TwoERPcomponents,referred toasearlyposterior negativ- ity(EPN)andlatepositivepotential(LPP),haveconsistentlybeen

DOIoforiginalarticle:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.04.006.

Correspondingauthorat:InstituteofPsychology,UniversityofKonstanz,POBox D36,78457Konstanz,Germany.Tel.:+497531882504;fax:+497531882971.

E-mailaddress:Harald.Schupp@uni-konstanz.de(H.T.Schupp).

foundtobemodulatedbyemotionalpicturevalence.TheEPNcom- ponentisobservedasarelativenegativedifferenceinprocessing emotionalpictures(pleasantandunpleasant)comparedtoneutral picturesovertemporo-occipitalsitesinatimewindowbetween 150and350ms(e.g.,Junghöferetal.,2001;Schuppetal.,2003, 2006).Thelatepositivepotential,measuredovercentro-parietal regions between300and 700ms, islargerfor emotional, com- paredtoneutral,stimuli(e.g.,Palombaetal.,1997;Schuppetal., 2000;Sabatinellietal.,2007).Studiesrelyingonstimulifromthe International Affective Picture System (IAPS, Lang et al., 2008) observed that EPN and LPP modulations are most accentuated for picturesdepictingevolutionarily relevantstimuluscontents, suchasreproduction-anddefense-relatedscenes(Schuppetal., 2003,2006),whichalsoprovokereliablesomatic,autonomicand humoralresponses(Bradleyetal.,2001).Recentresearchdemon- stratedtheemotionalmodulationoftheseERPcomponentsacross a broaderarray ofemotional stimuli includingemotional facial expressions(Mühlbergeretal.,2009;Satoetal.,2001;Schuppetal., 2004;Wieseretal.,2010),emotionalwords(Kissleretal.,2006), symbolicgestures(Flaischetal.,2009,2011),andclashingmoral statements(VanBerkumetal.,2009).Thesefindingshavebeencon- sideredfromtheperspectiveof‘naturalselectiveattention’,which holdsthatundernaturalisticconditionsstimulusperceptionand evaluationareoftendirectedbyunderlyingmotivationalsystems ofavoidanceandapproach(Bradley,2009;Langetal.,1997;Schupp etal.,2006).

Previous studies have mostly presented emotional stimuli in temporal isolation and used carefully planned presentation

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-228490

(2)

521

schemestoavoidconfoundsduetosequenceeffects(Flaischetal., 2008a,b).However,inthenaturalenvironment,stimulitypically donotoccurinisolation;instead,emotionalstimulimaycluster intimeandspace.Accordingly,peoplemayencountercongruent streamsorcontextsofpleasantorunpleasantstimuli.Thisraises thequestionofhowtherepeatedengagementofeithertheappe- titive oraversive motivational system affects theprocessing of individualemotionalcues.Onerecentstudyaddressedthisissueby presentingemotionalIAPSpictureseitherinanintermixedfashion orasstreamsof8picturesofthesamestimulustype(Pastoretal., 2008).ResultsindicatethatemotionalmodulationoftheLPPand positiveslowwavewascomparableinbothmixedandrepeated presentationconditions.Ontheotherhand,researchusingamod- ifiedoddballparadigmsuggeststhathedoniccontextsmayindeed modulateaffectivestimulusprocessing.Forinstance,positiveand negativepersonalityadjectiveselicitlargerLPPamplitudeswhen presented in a sequence ofadjectives of opposite valence con- texts than samevalence contexts(Cacioppoet al.,1993, 1994;

Critesetal.,1995).Anumberofmethodologicaldifferencesmay accountforthedivergentfindings obtainedinpreviousstudies.

Forinstance,studiesdifferedwithregardtothetypeofemotional stimuli(wordsvs.pictures)orthedurationofstimuluspresenta- tion(1.5vs.12s).Furtherresearchisthusneededtoelucidatethe effectsofhedoniccontextsuponemotionalstimulusprocessing.

Accordingly,thepresentstudyinvestigatedtheprocessingofemo- tionalpicturespresentedwiththeexperimentalparametersofthe modifiedoddballparadigm.

The mainaim of thepresent studies wasto investigatethe emotionalERPmodulation,indexedbyEPNandLPPcomponents, as a function of the valence of preceding images. To this end, stimuli were presented in streams of six pictures, in which a sequenceofpleasant,neutral,orunpleasantcontextpicturespre- cededpleasant,neutral, orunpleasanttargetpictures (i.e.,same valence context vs. opposite valence context).Studies 1 and 2 differedwithrespecttothepresentationofthehedoniccontext sequences.InStudy1,picturestreamscontainingdifferentvalence contextsalternatedrandomly(cf.Cacioppoetal.,1993).InStudy 2,60sequencesofeachvalencecontextwerepresentedinsucces- sion,counterbalancingtheirorderacrossparticipants.Asaresult, bothstudiesallowedacomparisonofemotionalpictureprocessing in the context of the same, a neutral, or the opposite valence picture sequence. Accordingly, the first aim was to determine whetherhedoniccontextmodulatesemotionprocessingindexed bytheEPNandLPPcomponent.Ontheonehand,ithasbeensug- gestedthat thediscriminationofemotionaland neutral stimuli mayrepresentanobligatoryprocess,whichisnotmodulatedby hedoniccontext(Pastoretal.,2008).Ontheotherhand,previous researchrevealedhedoniccontexteffectsonemotionprocessing (Cacioppoetal.,1993).Furthermore,severaldistincthypotheses maybederivedfrompreviousresearchregardingtheinteraction ofhedoniccontextandcurrentpictureprocessing.Forinstance, from an affective priming perspective, congruent valence con- textsmayfacilitatetheprocessingofemotionalpictures ifthey are of thesame valence as thehedonic context (cf.Avero and Calvo, 2006).Alternatively, ifanemotional pictureis incongru- entwiththehedoniccontext,itmaybeparticularlysalientand accordinglyefficientindrawingattentionalresources(Cacioppo et al., 1993).Furthermore, repeatedly presenting pictures from thesamevalencecategorymayleadtosensitizationeffects,par- ticularlyfortheprocessingofunpleasantimages, asseeninthe EMGcorrugatorresponse(Bradleyetal.,1996).Accordingly,ifthe contextwouldmodulateprocessingofcurrentemotionalstimuli, thesecondgoalofthestudywastorevealthedirectionof this modulationwithrespecttotheEPNandLPP components(gen- eralsensitizationvs.facilitationbythesameoroppositehedonic context).

2. Methods 2.1. Participants

ParticipantsinStudy1wereseventeen(11females)studentsfromtheUniver- sityofGreifswald.Participantswerebetweentheagesof19and26years.InStudy2, participantswereasecondgroupofseventeen(11females)studentsfromtheUni- versityofGreifswald,whowerebetweentheagesof19and27years.Allparticipants receivedcoursecredittowardstheirresearchrequirementsandprovidedwritten informedconsentfortheprotocol,whichwasapprovedbytheReviewBoardofthe UniversityofGreifswald.

2.2. Stimulusmaterialsanddesign

SixtytargetpicturesweretakenfromtheInternationalAffectivePictureSystem (IAPS;seeLangetal.,2008)depicting20unpleasantscenes(e.g.,spiders,muti- lations),20pleasantscenes(e.g.,attractiveinfants,oppositesexnudes),and20 neutralscenes(e.g.,neutralfaces,householdobjects).1Thethreecategoriesdif- feredsignificantlyfromeachotherintheirnormativevalenceratings(M=7.3,4.9, and2.3forpleasant,neutral,andunpleasantcontents,respectively;scalerange:

1–9).Meanarousallevelsforbothemotionalcategoriesweresignificantlyhigher thanforneutralcontents(M=5.4,2.8,and6.8forpleasant,neutral,andunpleasant contents,respectively).Thesetargetpictureswerethefocusoftheanalysisandwere presentedembeddedinasequenceofcontextpictures.

Adifferentsetof50pleasant,50neutral,and50unpleasantIAPSpictureswas selectedtocreatethehedoniccontexts.Thethreecontextcategoriesdifferedsig- nificantlyfromeachotherintheirnormativevalenceratings(M=7.2,5.4,and2.5 forpleasant,neutral,andunpleasantcontents,respectively).Meanarousallevels forbothemotionalcategoriesweresignificantlyhigherthanforneutralcontents (M=5.3,3.4,and5.8forpleasant,neutral,andunpleasantcontents,respectively).

Thepictureswerepresentedinstreamsof6pictures(cf.Cacioppoetal.,1993).

Fiveofthesixpicturesweredrawnfromthecontextpictureset.Thetargetpicture wasdrawnfromthetargetpicturesetandappearedthird,fourth,orfifthwithinthe picturestream.Presentingpicturesfromoneofthethreevalencecategoriesrealized thepleasant,neutral,orunpleasanthedoniccontext.Furthermore,picturessubse- quenttotargetpicturesvariedinvalencetoincreasethelikelihoodthatparticipants paidattentiontothepicturestreams(Cacioppoetal.,1993,1994).Atotalof180dis- tinctpicturestreamsoccurredsinceeachstimulusofthetargetpictureset(N=60) waspresentedwithineachofthethreehedoniccontextcategories(N=20foreach oftheninecontextbytargetvalencecombinations).Theparticipantspressedabut- tontoinitiateeachofthe180picturestreams.Eachpicturewasdisplayedfor1.5s andprecededbyawarningdot(.5s)toensurethattheparticipantswereattending tothescreen.Afterpictureoffset,participantswereaskedtoratethevalenceof thepicturesusingathree-wayresponsebutton.Theinter-trialintervalwas3s.One streamof6picturesservedasapracticetrial.

Study1and Study2 differedinthearrangement ofthehedoniccontext sequences.InStudy1,picturestreamscontainingpleasant,neutral,orunpleasant contextsequenceswerepresentedinrandomorder.InStudy2,contextpicture valencewasblockedsothatthesixtystreamsofeachcontextpicturevalence werepresentedinsuccession.Thisresultedin3consecutiveexperimentalblocks, eachconsistingof60picturestreamswithpleasant,neutral,orunpleasantcon- textsequences,respectively.Theorderofpresentationwascounterbalancedacross participantsinStudy2.

2.3. ERPrecordingsandanalysis

Brainandocularscalppotentialfieldsweremeasuredwitha129leadgeodesic sensornet,on-linebandpassfilteredfrom0.1to100Hz,andsampledat250Hz usingNetstationacquisitionsoftwareandEGIamplifiers(ElectricalGeodesics,Inc., Eugene,OR).Electrodeimpedancewaskeptbelow30k,asrecommendedforthis typeofamplifierbyEGIguidelines.Datawererecordedcontinuouslywiththever- texsensorasthereferenceelectrode.A35Hzdigitallowpassfilterwasapplied off-linetothecontinuousEEGdata.Single-trialepochswerecorrectedforvertical andhorizontaleyemovements(Milleretal.,1988).Dataeditingandartifactrejec- tionwerebasedonanelaboratemethodforstatisticalcontrolofartifacts,specifically tailoredfortheanalysesofdensesensorERPrecordings(Junghöferetal.,2000).Data werebaseline-corrected(100msprestimulus)andconvertedtoanaveragerefer- ence.Finally,separateaveragewaveformswerecalculatedforthe9experimental cells(TargetValencebyContextValence)foreachsensorandparticipant.Applying strictartifactcriteria,8.7(SD=1.6)and9.8(SD=1.4)trialswereusedtocalculate averagewaveformsinStudy1andStudy2,respectively.Trialnumberswerenot

1TheIAPSslidenumberswereasfollows:pleasant4670,4658,4660,4690,4650, 4680,4651,4652,4664,4659,2311,2341,2165,2170,2050,2080,2360,2070,2340, 2160:neutral– 2850,2570,2440,2480,2381,2230,2210,2200,2190,9070,7020, 7175,7235,7233,7010,7030,7080,7040,7002,7009:unpleasant1201,1120, 1300,1050,1930,3530,6510,6260,6350,6540,9405,3130,3080,3110,3060, 3102,3053,3000,3071,3010.

(3)

Fig.1. Illustrationofthestatisticalresults(p-valuesignificancemaps)ofthepoint- by-pointwaveformANOVA,averagedforillustrativepurposeacrossrelevanttime windowsfortheEPN(200–300ms;backview)andLPP(400–700ms;topview) componentforStudy1andStudy2,respectively.

differentforthetargetvalencecategoriesacrosshedoniccontexts.Acontrolanaly- siswithliberalartifactcriteria(meantrialnumber=17.3and17.6forStudy1and Study2,respectively)revealedcomparableresults.

Atwo-stepprocedurewasusedtoidentifyrelevantERPcomponents.These weresubsequentlyassessedbyconventionalERPanalysesbasedonareascores, i.e.,meanactivityinselectedsensorregionsandtimewindows.Inafirststep,visual inspectionandsinglesensorwaveformanalysiswereusedinconcerttoidentifyrel- evantERPcomponents.Toreplicatepreviousemotioneffectsandexplorehedonic contexteffects,singlesensorwaveformanalyseswerecalculatedforeachsensor andtimepointseparatelyincludingthefactorsTargetValence(pleasantvs.neutral vs.unpleasant)andContextValence(pleasantvs.neutralvs.unpleasant).Tocor- rectformultipletesting,effectswereonlyconsideredmeaningful,whentheeffects wereobservedforatleasteightcontinuousdatapoints(32ms)andtwoneighbor- ingsensors(SabbaghandTaylor,2000).Previousfindingsregardingtheemotional modulationoftheEPNandLPPcomponentwerefullyreplicated(seeFig.1).How- ever,neithersinglesensorwaveformanalysisnorvisualinspectionindicatedeffects involvinghedoniccontextvalence.

Inasecondstep,themeanactivityacrossselectedsensorsitesandtimebins wascalculatedtoscoreERPcomponents.WithregardtotheEPN,themeanactivity overatimeintervalfrom200to300mswascalculatedinleftandrighttemporo- occipitalsensorclusters(EGIsensornumbersoftheleftcluster:57,58,59,60,63, 64,65,66,67,69,70,71,72,74,75;rightcluster:77,78,83,84,85,86,89,90, 91,92,95,96,97,100,101).Duetovolumeconductioneffects,emotioneffectsover occipito-temporalareaswereobservedwithreversedpolarityoveranteriorsites(cf.

Schuppetal.,2003).Exploringthesecentro-frontalsensorsitesmirroredtheeffects observedfortheoccipitalnegativityand,forbrevity,willnotbereportedhere.To assesstheLPPcomponent,themeanactivityinleftandrightcentro-parietalsensor clusters(leftcluster:7,13,30,31,32,37,38,43,53,54;rightcluster:80,81,88,87, 94,105,106,107,112,113)wascalculatedinatimeintervalfrom400to700ms.

EPNandLPPdatawereenteredintoafour-factorialANOVAincludingthebetween- subjectsfactorStudy(Study1:randomcontextvs.Study2:blockedcontext)and thewithin-subjectsfactorsContextValence(pleasant,neutral,unpleasant),Target Valence(pleasant,neutral,unpleasant),andLaterality(left,right).

Foreffectsinvolvingrepeatedmeasures,theGreenhouse–Geisserprocedure wasusedtocorrectforviolationsofsphericity.

3. Results

3.1. EPNcomponent

ThemainfindingswithregardtotheEPNcomponentareillus- tratedinFigs.1and2.Thesinglesensorwaveformanalysisrevealed

Fig.2.IllustrationoftheEPNcomponentshowingarepresentativerightoccipi- talsensor(EGI#91)andscalppotentialmapsofthedifferencewaves‘Pleasant Neutral’,‘UnpleasantNeutral’forthethreehedoniccontextconditions(pleasant, neutral,andunpleasant)forStudy1(upperpanel)andStudy2(lowerpanel).Aback viewofthemodelheadisshown.

highlysignificanteffectsofTargetValenceoveroccipito-temporal regionsinbothStudy1andStudy2.Asinpreviousresearch,the differentialprocessingofemotionalascomparedtoneutralpic- tureswasseenasrelativenegativityoverposteriorregions.The effectisillustratedinFig.2withregardtothetemporalinforma- tionbydisplayingarepresentativerightoccipitalsensorandwith regardtothetopographicaldistributionbydisplayingdifference maps(pleasant–neutralandunpleasant–neutral)averagedacross atimeintervalfrom200to300ms.Mostimportantly,thesingle sensorwaveformanalysisprovidednoindicationofasignificant interactionbetweenTargetValenceand ContextValence.Specifi- cally,asshownalsoinFig.2,thehedoniccontext(pleasant,neutral, andunpleasantpictures)showednoreliableeffectsonemotional stimulusprocessingasindexedbytheEPNcomponent.Statistical analysisbasedonareascoresoveroccipito-temporalregionsina timewindowfrom200to300mscorroboratedthesefindings.

Replicatingpreviousfindings,ahighlysignificantmaineffect ofTargetValencewasobserved,F(2,64)=87.5,p<.0001.However, theeffectofTargetValencewasqualifiedbyasignificantinterac- tionofTargetValencebyStudyF(2,64)=3.8,p<.05.Accordingly, separateanalyseswereconductedforStudies1and2.Bothstud- iesrevealedahighlysignificantmaineffectofTargetValence,F(2, 32)=34.2and55.2,p’s<.001,forStudy1andStudy2,respectively.

PleasantimageselicitedalargerEPNcomponentcomparedtoneu- tralimages, t(16)=7.2 and 9.4,p’s<.0001.Similarly, unpleasant pictureselicitedalargerposteriornegativityascomparedtoneu- tralpictures,t(16)=5.3and5.0,p’s<.0001.Finally,asinprevious research,pleasantpicturesalsoelicitedamorenegativepotential comparedtounpleasantpictures,t(16)=2.9and6.6,p’s<.05.

Nofurthereffectreachedsignificanceinthisanalysis.Ofparticu- larinterest,neithertheinteractionTargetValence×ContextValence,

(4)

523

Fig.3.IllustrationoftheLPPcomponentshowingarepresentativecentralsensor (EGI#32)andscalppotentialmapsofthedifferencewaves‘PleasantNeutral’,

‘UnpleasantNeutral’forthethreehedoniccontextconditions(pleasant,neutral, andunpleasant)forStudy1(upperpanel)andStudy2(lowerpanel).Atopviewof themodelheadisshown.

F(4,128)=1.4,northeinteractionStudy×TargetValence×Context Valence, F(4, 128)=0.4, approached significance. Subsequent exploratory analyses were undertaken to examine the criti- calinteractionof TargetValence×ContextValenceseparately for bothstudies.TheinteractionTargetValence×ContextValence,F(4, 64)=0.3and0.7,wasnotsignificantinStudy1orStudy2.

3.2. LPPcomponent

ThemainfindingswithregardtotheLPPcomponentareillus- tratedinFigs.1and3.Theoutcomeofthesinglesensorwaveform revealedhighlysignificanteffects ofTargetValence overcentro- parietal regions for both studies. As in previous research, the differentialprocessingofemotional,comparedtoneutral,pictures wasseenasa positivepotentialshiftbetween400and700ms.

SimilartotheEPNcomponent,thesinglesensorwaveformanal- ysisdidnotrevealasignificantinteractionbetweenTargetValence andContextValence,neitherinStudy1,norinStudy2.Specifically, theemotionalLPPmodulationappearedsimilarlypronouncedin pleasant,neutral,andunpleasantcontextblocks.Tocorroborate thesefindings,theLPPcomponentwasscoredovercentro-parietal regionsina timewindowfrom400to700msandsubmittedto ANOVAanalysis.

AhighlysignificantmaineffectofTargetValencewasobserved, F(2,64)=98.2,p<.0001.Pleasantandunpleasantimageselicited anincreasedpositivity comparedtoneutral images, t(32)=10.9 and11.5,p’s<.0001,respectively.LPPstopleasantandunpleasant imageswerenotsignificantlydifferent,t(32)=0.7.

No further effect reached significance in this analysis. Nei- thertheinteractionTargetValence×ContextValence,F(4,128)=1.8, northeinteraction Study×TargetValence×ContextValence,F(4,

128)=1.3, approached significance. Furthermore, the separate analysisofStudies1and2revealednosignificantinteractionof TargetValence×ContextValence,F(4,64)=0.3and1.9,respectively.

4. Discussion

Emotionalstimuliarediscriminatedfromneutralcontentsearly intheprocessingstream.IthasbeensuggestedthattheEPNandLPP componentsreflectthemotivationalguidanceofselectivevisual attentiontowardsemotionalcues(Schuppetal.,2006).Bothstud- iesprovidedaone-to-onereplicationofpreviousfindingsregarding theselectiveprocessingofemotionalpictures.Between200and 300ms,pleasantandunpleasantpictureswereassociatedwitha relativenegativepotentialdifferenceoveroccipito-temporalsen- sorsitescomparedtoneutralpictures.Furthermore,between400 and700ms,theLPPcomponentovercentro-parietalsensorsites waslargerforemotionalcomparedtoneutralpictures.Thenovel findingofthepresentstudiesisthattheseeffectswereobserved regardlessofthevalenceoftheprecedingcontextpictures.Accord- ingly,theattentioncaptureofemotionalpicturesseemstodepend onthestimuluscharacteristicsoftheactualpictureirrespectiveof thehedoniccontextofthesequenceoftheprecedingpictures.

Thecurrentfindingsconfirmandextendpreviousfindingsthat ERPselicitedbyemotionalpicturesarenotaffectedbythehedonic context provided by preceding images. Pastor and colleagues (2008)presented emotionalpictures for 12swithalengthy ITI interval (12s) in which picturevalence varied eitherrandomly (group1)orwasrepeatedacross8presentations(group2).The mainfinding wasthattheLPP and positive slowwave compo- nentselicitedbyemotionalimagesweresimilarin randomand repeatedpresentations.Incomparison,theexperimentalparadigm usedinthepresentstudiesdifferedinseveralregards.Pictureswere presentedindistinct,self-pacedstreamsofsiximagesatfasterpre- sentationrates.Therepeatedpresentation(60streams)ofthesame hedoniccontextinStudy2shouldhavefacilitated theobserva- tionofhedoniccontexteffects.Furthermore,ratherthancomparing hedoniccontexteffectstoamixedseriesofemotionalandneutral pictures,thepresentstudypresentedemotionalpictureswithina hedoniccontextsequencewhichwaseitheraffectivelycongruent, incongruent,orneutral.Despitethesenotablemethodologicaldif- ferences,theLPPcomponentelicitedbyemotional(pleasantand unpleasant)imageswassimilarwhenpresentedinastreamincor- poratingaffectivelycongruentorincongruentcontextpictures.In addition,thesamepattern ofresultswasobservedwithregard totheEPNcomponent,whichreflectsarelativelyearlytransient processingstageatwhichemotionalstimuliaretaggedbasedon theirsignificance(Schuppetal.,2006).Whilethepatternoflarger EPNsto pleasant and unpleasant compared toneutral pictures (independentfromhedoniccontext)washighlysignificantinboth studies,theeffectappearedevenmorepronouncedinStudy2.This unexpectedfindingawaitsreplicationin futurestudies.Overall, thereis accumulatingevidencethat thediscriminationof emo- tionalandneutralstimulirepresentsanobligatoryprocessduring initialstimuluscategorization.

Hedoniccontexteffectsonemotionprocessinghavebeenexam- inedinaseparatelineofresearchusingrapidserialpresentation paradigms.Inonestudy,emotionalandneutralpictureswerepre- sentedinarapidstreamwithnointer-stimulusinterval(330ms;

Flaischetal.,2008a).Twomaineffectswereobserved.Asexpected, emotionalpictureselicitedalargerEPNcomponentcomparedto neutralimages.Ofparticularinterest,temporalinterferenceeffects wereobservedinthattheprocessingofthecurrentpicture(irre- spective of its valence) was attenuated when preceded by an emotional(pleasantorunpleasant)picture.Afurtherstudy,which presentedtheimagesfor660ms,revealedsimilareffectsforthe

(5)

LPPcomponent(Flaischetal.,2008b).Specifically,theLPPelicited bythecurrentpicturewasattenuatedwhenprecededbyanemo- tionalratherthanbyaneutralimage.Nointerferenceeffectswere observedfortheEPNorLPPcomponentinthepresentstudy,which presentedthepictureslonger(1.5s)andatslowerrate(ITI=3s).

Thus,thepresentfindingsprovideboundaryinformationregarding temporalinterferenceeffects.Competitionforprocessingresources betweenthecurrentandprecedingimagemaythusbelimitedto conditionswithhighdemandsonperceptualprocessing.

Theproblemsassociatedwiththeinterpretationofnull-findings needtobeacknowledged.Itispossiblethatlargersamplesmay revealhedoniccontexteffects.However,asthecurrentstudydid notobserveanyhedoniccontexteffects,itseemssafetoconclude that potentialhedoniccontext effects are minorin comparison tothepronouncedeffect observedfor targetpicturevalence in thepresentstudies.Furthermore,hedoniccontexteffectsmaybe observedbyrelyingonotherwaystocreatehedoniccontextorby usingdifferentstimulustypes(cf.Cacioppoetal.,1993,1994;Crites etal.,1995).Inthepresentstudies,hedoniccontextwascreatedby presentingpicturestimulifromavarietyofdistinctcategoriesof humanexperience(Bradleyetal.,2001).However,hedoniccontext effectsmaybeseenwhenimagesfromasinglesemanticcategory arepresented(i.e.,erotica,babies,mutilations,oranimalthreat).

Thestudyofhedoniccontexteffectsmayfurthermorebeinforma- tivewithregardtoclinicaldisorderssuchasthespectrumofanxiety disordersordrugaddiction.Previousstudieshavealreadyrevealed theenhancedprocessingofdrug-andfear-relevantstimuliindrug addictionandsmallanimalphobia,respectively(e.g.,Frankenetal., 2008; Michalowskiet al., 2009; Miltneret al.,2005; Kopp and Altmann, 2005).Therepeatedpresentationof disorder-relevant stimulimayprovidecomplementaryinformationregardingpos- sible sensitizationeffects todisorder-relevant stimuli revealing additionalneuralsystemsnotengagedinhealthysubjects.Over- all,whilethereisyetnoevidenceforhedoniccontexteffectson IAPSpictureprocessing,futurestudiesmayrevealsucheffectsby investigatingdifferentwaystocreatehedoniccontextandselected clinicalsamples.

IncontrasttotheERPfindings,previousresearchexamining hedoniccontext effects onemotionprocessing revealedsignifi- canteffects formotor responsemeasures.For instance,Bradley and colleagues (1996) presented continuous series of pleasant, neutral,orunpleasantpicturesandobservedsensitizationeffects for thecorrugator response duringunpleasant picture viewing.

Furthermore,Pastorandcolleagues(2008)observedthatcardiac deceleration,amotorindicatorofattentionalorienting,wasatten- uatedinblockedcomparedtomixedpicturepresentations.These studiesusedratherlongpicturepresentationtimesraisingtheissue whetherhedoniccontexteffectsformotorresponsesrelyonmore sustainedpictureprocessing.However,afurtherstudyobserved unpleasantcontexteffectsonthestartlereflexevenwhenthepic- tureswerepresentedasrapidandcontinuousstream(Smithetal., 2006).Accordingly,hedoniccontext effects maydiffer between measuresofperceptualprocessingandstimulusevaluationonthe one hand and somatic and autonomic motor responses onthe other(cf.Cacioppoetal.,1999).Abroaderperspectiveonemotion processing,includingtheconsiderationofthesocialcontextand themeasurementofcorticalandperipheral-physiologicalmeas- uresseemsinformativefortheunderstandingofpsychopathology, inparticulartheanxietyspectrum.Trackingtheflowofinforma- tionprocessingfrominitialstimulusevaluationtothemotoroutput stagewouldallowtodeterminethespecificstageofprocessingat whichstimulusprocessingoffearedobjectsissensitizedbyantic- ipatoryanxiety.

Overall,thereisincreasingevidencethattheemotionalguid- ance ofattentionis dictatedbytheinformation depictedinthe current stimulus. Studies investigating the effects of repeated

stimulus exposure and hedonic context support this hypothe- sis (Codispoti et al., 2007; Ferrari et al., 2011; Flaisch et al., 2008a,b;Pastoretal.,2008;Schuppetal.,2006).Whileimplicit andinstructedstimulussignificanceareoftenassociatedwithsim- ilar ERP modulations in terms of an occipital negativityand a latepositivepotential,arecentstudyobserveddistincteffectsfor bothmechanismsofattentionregulation(BublatzkyandSchupp, 2011).Usingtheinstructedfearparadigm,participantswerever- ballyinstructedthattheymight receiveanelectricshockwhen a specificemotional (pleasant or unpleasant) or neutral image cuewaspresented. Instructedfear signalselicited a ratherdis- tinctERPsignature(P1,P2,andLPP)whichwasindependentfrom the EPN and LPP modulations associated with emotional stim- ulus contents. Thus, whilestimulus detectiontasks oftenshow similar ERP modulations to emotion processing (Ferrari et al., 2008;Schuppetal.,2007),intrinsicandinstructedstimulussig- nificancecanalsobedissociatedfromeachotheratthelevelof stimulusencoding.Theobligatorynatureoftheattentioncapture byemotionalcuesmayalsohaveimplicationsforpsychotherapy research.Specifically, increasedselectiveattentiontopathology relevantcuesisoftenobserved(e.g.,increasedselectiveattention tophobia-relevantstimuli)andinterpretedasanattentionprob- lem(Mathewsand MacLeod,1994).However,theregulationof thefacilitated processingofemotionalandtask-relevantstimuli ispresumablyatleastinpartsubservedbydistinctneuralcircuits.

Emotionalstimuliengagecorticalandsubcorticallimbicstructures (LangandDavis,2006),which,inadditiontomodulatingcortical attentionsystems(PosnerandPeterson,1990),mayregulateper- ceptualprocessinginvisualcortexbydirectprojections (Emery and Amaral, 2000) or via ascending neuromodulatory systems (DerryberryandTucker,1991).Consideringsuchneuraldifferences interms ofattentionalcontrolmechanismsmayhelptounder- standwhycognitiveinterventionsattemptingtochangeselective attentiontopathologyrelevantcuesmightnotbeaseffectivethan stimulusdrivenapproaches (e.g.,exposurebased interventions) aimingtochangetheemotionalimpactofthesecuesbyextinction learning.

Acknowledgements

ThisresearchwassupportedbytheGermanResearchFounda- tion(Schu1074/10-3and1074/11-2).WewouldliketothankDr.

JessicaStockburgerforhelpindataacquisitionandanalysis.

References

Avero,P.,Calvo,M.G.,2006.Affectiveprimingwithpicturesofemotionalscenes:

theroleofperceptualsimilarityandcategoryrelatedness.SpanishJournalof Psychology9,10–18.

Bradley,M.M.,Cuthbert,B.N.,Lang,P.J.,1996.Picturemediaandemotion:effectsof asustainedaffectivecontext.Psychophysiology33,662–670.

Bradley,M.M.,Codispoti,M.,Cuthbert,B.N.,Lang,P.J.,2001.Emotionandmotivation I:defensiveandappetitivereactionsinpictureprocessing.Emotion1,276–298.

Bradley,M.M.,Sabatinelli,D.,Lang,P.J.,Fitzsimmons,J.R.,King,W.M.,Desai,P.,2003.

Activationofthevisualcortexinmotivatedattention.BehavioralNeuroscience 117,369–380.

Bradley, M.M.,2009. Naturalselective attention:orienting andemotion. Psy- chophysiology46,1–11.

Bublatzky,F.,Schupp,H.T.,2011.Picturescueingthreat:braindynamicsinview- ingexplicitlyinstructeddangercues.SocialCognitive&AffectiveNeuroscience, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr032.

Cacioppo,J.T.,Crites,S.L.,Berntson,G.G.,Coles,M.G.,1993.Ifattitudesaffecthow stimuliareprocessedshouldtheynotaffecttheevent-relatedbrainpotential?

PsychologicalScience4,108–112.

Cacioppo,J.T.,Crites,S.L.,Gardner,W.L.,Berntson,G.G.,1994.Bioelectricalechoes fromevaluativecategorizations:I.Alatepositivebrainpotentialthatvariesas afunctionoftraitnegativityandextremity.JournalofPersonalityandSocial Psychology67,115–125.

Cacioppo,J.T.,Gardner,W.L.,Berntson,G.G.,1999.Theaffectsystemhasparal- lelandintegrativeprocessingcomponents:formfollowsfunction.Journalof PersonalityandSocialPsychology76,839–855.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

The increased sensory processing of emotional compared to neutral stimuli was observed in several fMRI- and PET studies [10,11], including materials such as pleasant or

Similarly, increased LPP amplitudes to pleasant and unpleasant relative to neutral images would indicate late selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures..

1.) a fast and rapidly acting sensitized brainstem-amygdala-cortical alarm system. The early activation of this system commences a successive prioritized information

Pleasant highly arousing pictures in terms of valence, irrespective of their arousal degree, were associated with enhanced accuracy rates compared to the unpleasant highly

Revealing a valence- specific effect of anticipatory anxiety on affective picture viewing, facilitated processing of pleasant cues was observed during threat- of-shock compared

&amp; 2) appeared on the screen for 400 ms. The following three time windows were used to evaluate the ERP response to these stimuli. 01) was found, showing that the amplitude of

Affective modulation of picture recognition Participants will recog- nize pleasant and unpleasant pictures better than neutral ones, when the pictures are presented for only a

The middle panel shows the main effect of prime picture valence on the subsequent target picture processing by collapsing ERP waveforms across pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant