• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Energetics of the acrobatic courtship in male golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Energetics of the acrobatic courtship in male golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus)"

Copied!
8
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Subject Areas:

behaviour, physiology, ecology

Keywords:

energetics, heart rate telemetry, courtship, tropics, manakins

Author for correspondence:

J. Barske

e-mail: jbarske@ucla.edu

Energetics of the acrobatic courtship in male golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus)

J. Barske

1

, L. Fusani

3

, M. Wikelski

4,5

, N. Y. Feng

2

, M. Santos

6,7

and B. A. Schlinger

1,2

1Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, and2Department of Integrative Biology and Physiology, and the Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology, Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA

3Department of Biology and Evolution, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy

4Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Radolfzell, Germany

5Konstanz University, Konstanz, Germany

6Departamento de Entomologı´a Me´dica, Instituto Conmemorativo Gorgas de Estudios de la Salud y Instituto Smithsonian de Investigaciones Tropicales, Panama

7Instituto Smithsonian de Investigaciones Tropicales, Panama

In lek mating systems, females choose mates through indicators of quality, which males may exhibit by their performance of courtship displays. In tem- perate regions, displaying seasons are brief (one to two months), whereas in the tropics courtship seasons may be prolonged. Moreover, in temperate- breeding animals lekking behaviour can be energetically demanding, but little is known about the energy costs of lekking in tropical animals. Daily, over the course of a nearly seven-month-long breeding season, male golden- collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus) of Panamanian rainforests perform acrobatic courtship displays that markedly elevate heart rates, suggesting that they require high energy investment. Typically, animals of tropical low- land forests (such as manakins) exhibit a ‘slow pace of life’ metabolic strategy. We investigated whether male manakin courtship is indeed metabo- lically costly or whether the birds retain a low daily energy expenditure (DEE), as seen in other tropical species. To assess these questions, we calibrated man- akin heart rate against metabolic rate, examined daily lek activity and, using telemetry, obtained heart rates of individual wild, lekking male manakins.

Although metabolic rates peak during courtship displays, we found that males actually invest minimal time (only approx. 5 min d 1) performing displays. As a consequence, the DEE of approximately 39 kJ d 1for male man- akins is comparable to other lowland tropical species. The short, intense bursts of courtship by these birds make up only approximately 1.2% of their total DEE. Presumably, this cost is negligible, enabling them to perform daily at their arenas for months on end.

1. Introduction

Males of many species provide no direct benefits to their mates, nor help with par- ental care, but aggregate in leks to court females for purposes of copulating [1].

Under these conditions, females choose a partner among many males, pre- sumably assessing indicators of quality, such as male ornaments, behaviour or position in the lek [1,2]. Such intensive male male competition and/or selective female choice can lead to the evolution of exaggerated secondary sexual pheno- typic and/or behavioural traits [3,4]. In temperate regions, lekking species may display across a one- to two-month-long season when the males are thought to expend considerable energy in courtship, here defined as time on the lek when many individual ‘courtship displays’ are performed. In birds, for example, male great snipe (Gallinago media) [5] and sage grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus) [6] spend many hours per day at their leks. Their courtship activity increases their metabolic rates, causing significant weight loss in some individuals (usually the less successful males) [6]. The large investment in courtship, however, can be

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-0-262373

Erschienen in: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B : Biological Sciences ; 281 (2014), 1776. - 20132482

(2)

rewarded by high reproductive success, as a few males monopolize most matings [1,7].

Many species of lekking birds live in the tropics [1], but studies of their energy investment in courtship are scarce. Com- pared with birds breeding at high latitudes, lowland tropical birds tend to develop slowly, invest less in reproduction each year, and display slow senescence and extended longevity, as well as a low metabolic rate; a series of life-history traits that collectively reflect a tropical ‘slow pace of life’ [8 17]. By tropical, we refer to the wet, lowland forest environment, as different metabolic life-history strategies appear to apply to tropical animals living at high elevations or in deserts [18].

Golden-collared (GC) manakins (18.1 g;Manacus vitellinus) are tropical passerine birds that are known for their elaborate courtship displays. GC manakins live in lowland forests of Panama. During a lengthy breeding season (up to seven months in duration), adult males spend much of the day at their leks engaged in male male and male female inter- actions that include performance of courtship displays.

Courtship is dominated by the ‘jump snap’ display, which involves vigorous and rapid jumping between young saplings interspersed with loud snaps [19 21]. Individual ‘wingsnaps’

as well as high frequency ‘rollsnaps’ are produced as the wings strike one another after being lifted rapidly over the birds’ back [22,23]. When displaying, heart rates are profoundly elevated in males, even briefly exceeding 1300 beats min 1 [24], one of the highest heart rates recorded for any avian/mammalian species [25,26]. This elevated heart rate suggests that male manakins expend considerable energy in the performance of courtship activities. Outside of courtship, however, GC mana- kins exhibit life-history traits typical of the ‘slow pace’ of tropical life, producing small clutches (one to two eggs [10,27]) and living as long as 14 years [9,28,29]. Thus, it is unclear whether, during the breeding season, male GC mana- kins are able to sustain a tropical slow pace of life in the face of the putative high energy demands of courtship or whether GC manakins are an exception to the general rule that tropical birds have a lower energy expenditure than temperate birds.

Efforts to assess organismal energy metabolism have used various measures that describe overlapping and distinct states of metabolism, including basal (BMR) and resting metabolic rate (RMR), as well as peak metabolic rate (PMR), which are determined under controlled conditions in the laboratory [11,30,31]. Field metabolic rate (FMR), or daily energy expendi- ture (DEE) is used to assess the average metabolic rate of an animal under natural conditions. Whereas the tropical

‘slow pace of life’ is associated with a relatively low BMR and PMR [11 17], there are insufficient data to assign a defini- tive FMR measure for lowland tropical birds, though a trend towards decreased FMR has been described [18].

Although these measures describe average energy expendi- ture, they are less useful for assessing energy demands of discrete behaviours. One can determine the absolute amount of energy required for a given behaviour by accounting for the duration of the behaviour (‘joule cost’) or as the increase in energy expenditure per unit time with an emphasis on the peak demand required ( joules per sec; ‘power’ [32]). The latter measure is usually applied to behaviours involving rapid bursts of high energy. During a chase, for example, chee- tahs are thought to display a brief 50-fold increase in their RMR [33]. This behaviour requires high power but low joule cost.

By contrast, while running, a marathoner displays a minimal increase in metabolic rate, but for an extended time period.

This involves a high joule cost but less power [34,35]; ‘joule cost’ is not to be confused with the evolutionary ‘cost of a trait’ [3,4,36]. Both types of behaviour might require the animal to develop specialized physiological and morphologi- cal adaptations, one for bouts of PMR and one for extended metabolic demands (reviewed in [37,38]). Such distinctions are especially valuable for understanding energy expenditure of discrete behaviours such as courtship.

Gathering accurate data on energy utilization of natural animal behaviours can be difficult, so captive animals are often studied, as was the case for cheetahs described above (by measuring O2consumption on captive cats breathing into venti- lated masks while running on treadmills [33]). Using telemetry and miniature transmitters, heart rate measures can be obtained from wild animals. They provide excellent temporal resolution, such that estimates of metabolic rate can be obtained in the order of seconds to minutes. This contrasts with more traditional tech- niques (e.g. the doubly labelled water method) that provide estimates across days [39 42]. Heart rate stands as a proxy for energy expenditure [42], so the application of heart rate teleme- try can be a valuable technique to identify moment-to-moment energy expenditure [3,4,36].

Previous studies of GC manakins obtained estimates of BMR that suggested that they maintain metabolic rates com- parable to other tropical species that lack vigorous courtship displays [12,43,44]. As the relationship between BMR and DEE is not clearly defined [45,46], we sought to obtain diur- nal and nocturnal measures of energy expenditure of wild lekking males during their peak months of courtship. Heart rate telemetry, in combination with respirometry to estimate energy expenditure, enabled us for the first time to obtain estimates of energy expenditure during various hours of the day, as well as during specific courtship behaviours in free- living birds, something not previously possible (cf. [47,48]).

Energy expenditure was estimated in relation to measures of daily lek activity as well as focal observations of individual males (this study and [24]). In addition, we compared these estimates to comparable data obtained previously from a similar, though non-lekking, tropical bird, the spotted ant- bird (Hylophylax naevioides) [49]. Our work shows that male manakins exhibit a tropical slow pace of life despite the per- formance of energetically costly displays. These data offer new insight into the energetics of tropical courtship.

2. Material and methods

Studies were conducted during the dry season (February March) in forests around Gamboa, Panama (9870N, 798420W) and in facilities maintained by the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (STRI).

(a) Heart rate telemetry

Heart rates were recorded via telemetry using miniature transmit ters as described previously for small birds [47,49 51], including GC manakins [24]. Using mistnets, eight adult male GC manakins were captured from five leks. Birds were given coloured leg bands for individual identification. Out of eight birds, four were trans ferred to a laboratory facility within 10 km of their capture (and release) site, where transmitters were attached and respirometry calibration was performed. On the remaining four birds, transmit ters were attached in the field and birds were released within approximately 10 min at the site of capture. Each transmitter (approx. 1 g; Sparrow Systems, Fisher, USA) has a 9 cm flexible

2

(3)

wire antenna and two wire electrodes, and was glued to trimmed feathers on the centre of the bird’s back. The two electrodes were inserted beneath the skin (for details, see [49,50]). The heart rate transmitters emitted a continuous, amplitude modulated carrier signal that was frequency modulated by heart muscle potentials.

Transmitters attached in this way fall off after approximately 14 days, eliminating the need for recapture of the birds [52].

Every 10 min for 30 s, using a Yagi antenna connected to an AR8000 receiver (AOR Ltd., Japan), heart rates were recorded as sound files (mp3) on a digital recorder connected to the receiver (mp3 recorder, Edirol R09, Roland Inc., Japan). In this way, heart rate measures were obtained randomly with respect to behaviour, providing an accurate reflection of the bird’s daily commitment to different energetic demands. Sound files were analysed using COOLEDITv. 2000 software (Syntrillium Software, USA). Bandpass filters (Fast Fourier transform filters) were applied to increase the signal to noise ratio between heart rate frequency and carrier fre quency. Heart rate was measured by identifying 10 consecutive amplitude spikes, and then averaging the time intervals between each spike pair [51]; skeletal muscle potentials were irregular and easily distinguished from iterative potentials emanating from the heart.

(b) Calibration of heart rate versus O

2

consumption

Energy metabolism is assumed to be equivalent to O2consump tion, as aerobic respiration restocks depleted ATP. Fick’s equation relates heart rate to O2consumption using stroke volume and the arterio venous difference in O2content, the product of which is defined as O2pulse (OP) [53]. If OP is constant or varies in a pre dictable manner, a calibration between O2 consumption and heart rate can define this relationship [42,54]. If CO2production is known, the respiratory exchange ratio (RER) of O2consump tion and CO2production can be calculated in order to estimate metabolic rate [55]. Heart rate is not a measure of absolute energy expenditure as a potential error is associated with the metabolic rate estimates based on the RER obtained in the cali bration equation [42,54]. Heart rate was calibrated against O2

consumption in four male GC manakins. Once fitted with a transmitter, the male was placed in an open flow respirometer consisting of an airtight plastic 2 l volume chamber that enabled simultaneous measurement of O2inflow and CO2outflow. Flow rate was adjusted to 1400 ml min 1, allowing for 95% equili brium within 1.26 min or 99% equilibrium within 3.3 min, and ambient temperatures were 28+18C. Heart rate was averaged across the 5 s preceding the respirometry measurement, after ensuring that heart rate was stable in the 60 s preceding the measurements. Birds could freely move around in the chamber, but when placed in a dark location, they were generally quies cent in the chambers. If individuals did not move vigorously to achieve high heart rates, we approached the chambers and birds were enticed to move. For each bird, RER was determined at seven to eight different heart rates. Instantaneous O2consump tion was calculated using the equation of Bartholomewet al. [56], after empirically determining its denominator. Following With ers [57], we obtained the rate of O2consumption (for detailed methodological description, see [47,57]). These measurements were then used to obtain an equation relating heart rate to metabolic rate as described below.

(c) Comparative analyses of manakin energetics

Heart rate measures and estimates of DEE were compared with those obtained using a similar method in another lowland tropi cal bird of similar size, the spotted antbird (17.6 g) [49]. From six adult male antbirds, heart rates were recorded continuously and measurements were obtained every 5 min for 24 h (for additional details, see [49]). Additionally, we compared GC manakin DEE with estimates derived from several other models [18,58,59].

GC manakin minimal MR, the best estimate of BMR or RMR in wild birds in which environmental conditions and feeding status cannot be controlled, was compared with RMR estimates obtained from the equation of Aschoff & Pohl [60].

(d) Courtship activity

In a previous study, we found that courtship displays (in particular the production of mechanical wing sounds) are associated with extremely high heart rates, hence they are the most energy demanding behaviours performed on the lek [24]. Pointing to their importance, wingsnap and rollsnap frequencies are also sig nificant predictors of mating success [24]. We calculated average on lek activity across the day using audio recordings (06.30 to 17.30). During our study period, sunrise occurred between 06.03 and 06.34, with sunset occurring between 18.10 and 18.35. A microphone attached to a digital recorder with more than 12 h of battery life was placed in a central position within five different leks (with 4+1 adult courting males) from which we obtained three 1 day audio recordings (see electronic supplementary material, table S1). To estimate daily social and courtship activity, audio files were analysed using COOLEDITv. 2000 software. Divid ing the file into 1 h intervals, starting at 06.30, we counted the number of wingsnaps and rollsnaps between 06.30 and 07.30, which represented activity at 07.00, and so on. To estimate activity of individual males on a lek, we divided the hourly sums of mech anical sounds produced by the number of males occupying that lek. Breeding male GC manakins have very high lek attendance and on the day of recording, all males were known to be present on the lek. In addition, we used data from previously obtained video recordings (n¼17) and focal observations (n¼31) of indi vidual males, to estimate mating success in relation to the time spent by males performing courtship displays [24]. High speed videos (n¼87) of 17 courting males were used to measure the average duration of a courtship display. With focal observations, we measured the number of displays and rollsnaps performed by males as well as their mating success.

(e) Statistical analyses

SPSS v. 19 (IBM Inc., USA) was used for statistical analyses.

Calibration relationships between heart rate and O2consumption of birds in the respirometer were determined by least square regressions. A generalized linear model (GLM) with metabolic rate as the dependent variable, individual as a random factor and heart rate as a covariate was used to determine the overall relationship between heart rate and O2consumption based on the data collected for the four birds during calibration. The stan dard error of the estimate (SEE) and the coefficient of variation (COV¼100SEE/estimate) were calculated after Green [54].

To investigate the pattern of courtship activity, we used a GLM with hour of the day as the repeated measure (daytime) and leks as the between subject factor. The pattern of heart rate across the day was examined using a GLM with hour of day as the repeated measure (daytime). Heart rates were log transformed to obtain normality.Post hoctests with Bonferroni corrections were used to identify differences of activity or heart rate between different time points. Differences between antbirds and GC manakins were investigated usingttests. If not indicated differently, values in the text are given as mean+s.e.m.

3. Results

(a) Patterns of daily courtship activity

Activity levels varied significantly across the day and also between leks (daytime: F3.5,34.6¼9.159, p,0.001; lek:

F4,10¼5.643,p¼0.012; interaction:F13.8,34.6¼1.743,p¼0.266;

3

(4)

1

150

"1125

<a 8 100 .§

~

75

e

] 50

c btJ 25

·~

7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 time of day (b)

Figure 1.

Diurnal pattern

of

courtship activity of male GC manakins.

Mechanically produced sounds

{wingsnaps

and rollsnaps),

measu~s

of man- akin social and courtship behaviour at the lek,

we~

recorded for frve different leks in

th~e 1~ay ~ordings.

Hourly means were cakulated including 30 min

befo~

and after

the time

point. Values

we~ then

divided by the number of males

{4

±

1)

on

the

corresponding lek, which was determined with

di~ct

behavioural observations. Mean values of sounds produced male

1

h

1

on each of the frve leks

a~

shown.

heart rate

9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

9 10

~ 11

·;;:

·.c t) 12

<U

13 14 15 16

17

~ ~ ~

Figure 2.

Hourly

diffe~nces

in activity and heart rate levels. Results of Bonferroni-<:orrected post hoc tests comparing activity and heart rate levels between

diffe~nt

hoUis of the day

{07.00 to 17.00).

Arrows indicate signifi- cant

diffe~nces.

The shaded

a~a

shows values for heart

rates and arrows

~fer

to

the hours on the top. For example, heart rate is lower at 13.00 com-

pared with 08.00. The non-shaded area

of the table

refers

to

activity and arrows

~fer to hours on the left. An

example is the significantly lower activity at 17.00 compared with 07.00 and 08.00.

figure 1). Thus, despite differences between leks in the magni- tude of activity, leks were similar with respect to their patterns of daily courtship. Activity was generally elevated in the morning, declining to a nadir at 12.00, after which activity levels increased significantly to a peak at 14.00, and then declined significantly by 17.00 (figure 2).

(b ) Patterns of daily heart rate

Telemetry transmitters added approximately 5.5% of a bird's body mass (18.1 g) and appeared to have minimal impact on the birds: males with transmitters were regularly observed

1

.s

750

8 500

e

~

~

250

~

..,

.c

06.00 12.00 18.00

time (h)

F

igure 3

. Daily heart rate pattern. The thick line rep~ents the hourly mean

heart rates

of eight male GC manakins. Hourly averages were calculated for

the 30 min

befo~

and after the time point. During

the

night, we

obtained

heart rate measurements of only six manakins. No

measu~

was taken at 3.00 in

the morning, thus

we averaged values of 2.00 and 4.00

to complete the

graph.

Thin

lines

~present the 95% Cls {1 s.d.).

Shaded areas indicate night

time (18.12

until 06.15).

courting and sometimes even copulating with females (see the electronic supplementary material, video S1, of [24]). Never- theless, we cannot rule out the possibility that they have some impact on the birds, including increasing energy expendi- ture, leading to an overestimation of natural energy expenditure (reviewed in [61]).

For six birds, we measured heart rates across a romplete 24 h span; heart rates during daylight hours were measured on two additional birds (61 ± 12 diurnal recordings veiSUS 17 ± 4 noc- turnal recordings; electronic supplementary material, table 52).

When comparing hourly individual averages between 07.00 and 17.00, we found a significant effect of daytime on heart rate (F10;10

=

6.180, p < 0.001; figure 3). Heart rates rose steeply after 05.00, reaching a significant peak of 596 ± 37 beats min 1 at 07.00, where they remairled for another hour (figure 2).

Following this early morning peak, heart rates significantly decreased to 394± 14 beats min 1 at 13.00. For the remainder of the afternoon, heart rates were slightly elevated, reaching a peak at 18.00 (500 ± 16 beats min 1), a time that rorresponded with decreased activity on the lek (figure 2).

At sunset, heart rates markedly declined, with nocturnal rates generally low, decreasing to a min of 290

±

8 beats min 1 at 04.00. This result rorresponds with previous findings of low noc- turnal metabolic activity in GC manakins such that the birds appear to exhibit heterothermia, achieving a minimum nocturnal body temperature of 26.8°C [44]. Nevertheless, GC manakins retain the capacity to be responsive at night [44] as we observed discrete surges in nocturnal heart rate (approx. 700 beats min 1)

in one bird, likely to indicate reactions to disturbance.

Taken together, these data allow us to calculate mean diurnal (active) heart rate of 450

±

15 beats min 1, calcula-

ted from 06.00 to 18.30, and a mean nocturnal heart rate of 332

±

23 beats min 1 Overall, the mean daily heart rate (24 h period) was 398± 13 beats min 1

( c ) Calibration of heart rate versu s 0

2

consumption

Heart rate calibration measurements sparmed 382 943 beats min 1 (see electronic supplementary material, table S3), coinciding roughly with the range of heart rates recorded in the field (195 979 beats min 1). During courtship displays,

11

(5)

L40 120

~100

'0

g

80

G)

«< ....

0 60

:g .,

~ 40

Q)

8 20 0

350

. ., ..

... · .,. , .

• .,• - E

.. .

.. · ·"

. - .tlf/IIJ - - G

.. ., . .,

., .. ··-·H

--- 1

450 550 650 750 850 950 heart rate (beats min-1)

F

igure 4.

Relationship between heart rates and metabolic rates. least-square regressions of heart rates against metabolic rates for four birds determined by calibration measurements: bird E

(p < 0.001),

bird G

(p 0.013),

bird H

(p < 0.001)

and bird I

(p <

0.001).

See

table 1 for

statistical

details.

average heart rates exceeded these ranges by 38 beats min 1 [24], so there may be some error in estimating metabolic rate at those elevated heart rates, as calibration of such high energy levels cannot be obtained in the laboratory. The RER was on average 0.88 ± 0.01, which lies within the predicted range of 0.7 1 [30].

A linear relationship between 02 consumption and heart rate of captive birds is required to estimate energy expenditure from heart rates recorded in the wild. We found such a relation- ship in GC manakins (figure 4 and table 1), confirming what had been observed previously in other small birds (47,49,51].

When comparing the regressions obtained from the four birds, we found that intercepts were significantly different between individual regressions {F4;2a

=

11.77, p < 0.001), but the slopes were the same (table 1). Following Green et al. [411 we obtained a final regression equation using the slope from the pooled data with an average of the individual inter- cepts to predict energy expenditure in the wild: MR

=

{fH x 0.148) 19.796, where MR is the metabolic rate in kJ day 1 and /H is the heart rate in beats min 1. Eqn (11) from [41]

was used to calculate the standard error of the estimated MR. The error associated with the variation between GC manakins (d2) was 48.5

(kJ

2)(beats min 1) 2 and the error associated with the scatter around the regression lines (e2)

was 8.1 x 10 5 (beats min 1

f

(d ) D aily e n ergy m etabolis m a n d comp arative a n alyses

From the average daily heart rate, we estimated DEE to be 39.1 ± 2.5 kJ day 1 (table 2). We are confident of these estima- tes given the relatively low 4 6% COV we obtained for each estimate. The lowest recorded nocturnal heart rate was used to estimate minimal MR, calculated to be 14.1 ±4.7 kJ day 1.

Manakins had significantly lower day {t12

=

5.534,

p

< 0.001) and 24 h {t10 = 4.525,

p

= 0.001) heart rates

than those of spotted antbirds but no difference was found in DEE between the two species (t10

=

0.609, p

=

0.556;

table 2). Similar oxygen consumption combined with a lower heart rate suggests that manakins have an increased cardiac output, that is they eject a greater volume of blood with each heartbeat. This conclusion is supported by the lim- ited available data on heart weight in these species. A higher cardiac output is typically associated with a larger heart mass;

Table

1.

Parameters of least-square regressions

for

heart rate

against

g

metabolic rate of four male golden-collared manakins. Birds

equipped

with

a heart rate transmitter

were placed in a respirometer, and heart rate was measured simultaneously with

02

consumption and

C02

production.

The

RER is used

to estimate

metabolic rate, following Walsberg & Hoffman

[55].

bird, birds used

for

calibrations; n, number

of

measurements

that

have been

taken at different heart rates;

a, intercept;

b,

slope.

bird

n a

b

r

p

E 7

13.8

0.15 0.97 < 0.001

... , ... ... .. .. ... . ... ··· .... .. . ...

G 7

4.76

0.14 0.71 0.017

H 7

22.55

0.15 0.93 <0.001

... ,,,, .. ,,,,,,,, ... .

8 35.18 0.15 0.94 <0.001

when heart weight to body weight ratios are calculated for these two species (dry heart weights over wet body weights from [62]), that of manakins (0.046/15 g = 3.1 x 10 ~exceeds that of antbirds (0.044/16.1 g

=

2.7 x 10 ~.

Our estimates of DEE in manakins obtained from heart rate measures compare favourably with those based on a gen- eral model for tropical birds [18], whereas estimates obtained from models for passeriformes in general [59] or songbirds from arid and mesic climates [58] were almost twice that of the GC manakins (table 3). Moreover, applying the equation of Aschoff & Pohl [60] that estimates BMR of temperate zone birds, a manakin~ized bird is predicted to have a BMR of 26 kJ day 1, a value twice the minimal MR of 14 kJ day 1 that we estimated in this study.

(e) E n erg y r e q u iremen ts for the cou rtsh ip d isplay

Male GC manakin heart rates rose during the perlormance of courtship displays and rollsnaps to 1017 ± 28 and 1062 ± 102 beats min 1, respectively (24]. For courtship displays, this rate corresponded to 131 kJ day 1 (5.5 kJ h 1) with a COV of 3% (corrected from [24]), which is equivalent to the PMR of manakins induced by exercise measured by Wiersma et al.

[11]. Although overalllek activity lasted for many hours per day, males varied greatly in the total number of displays they performed, ranging from 1.6 to 140 displays day 1, producing an average of 32±5 displays day 1 (n

=

31). Importan-

tly, each display lasts on average 9.5 ± 0.5 s (n

=

17), thus

males engage in courtship displays for only 5

±

1 min day 1 (0.25 222 min day \ data from [24]). Consequently, they spent on average 0.46 kJ in courtship displays, which cor- responds to 12% of their DEE. On average, males also produce 10.8± 1.5 rollsnapsh 1, expending an additional 0.04 kJ day 1 on rollsnaps. Thus, the performance of the two most demanding courtship behaviours requires on average 0.5 kJ day 1. If we assume that the power required for a court- ship display is similar across males, then the least active males would spend approximately 0.1% of their DEE in courtship displays compared with approximately 52% of the most active ones (see electronic supplementary material, figure 51).

Although we have not measured energy investment in court- ship in relation to mating success in GC manakins, it is likely that from least active to most active and successful males, GC manakins expend a minimal amount of energy in their court- ship displays including the production of mechanical sounds.

(6)

Table 2

.

Heart

rate measures and estimates of

DEE

of

two tropical birds. DEE obtai ned with heart

rate telemetry was compar

ed between the golden-collared g

manak in (18. 1 g) and the spotted antbird (17.6 g). Col umns 2 and 3 a

re mean values for manak

ins and antbi rds fo

r the following variables: average diurnal

heart

rate (day), average minimal heart rate (night), average daily heart rate (24 h) and average DEE. M

easures are reported as mean ± s .e. (sample size).

Columns 4 and

5 show the results of t-tests comparing the different variables between the two species. Manakins have lower heart rates for two of the three

measures, but no difference is found in DEE. Also

w

hen comparing mass corrected DEE,

w

e did not find a significant difference. Data on antbirds are taken from

[49).

variable manakin antbird t (d.f.)

p

day (b m in

1)

450 ± 15 (8) 601 ± 24 (6 ) 5

.534(12)

0

.000

····~~h't''(b'~i~···;·i'""""""''"''""""""""''i3i'±'i3''(6i'

384± 15 (6) 1.896(10) 0

.087

''''24'i;''(b'~i~'''ii''''""'''''"''''''''''''''''''"''''397±'i'i''(6) 492

±

17 (6)

4

.525(10)

0.001

··iiu ..

i~··d~y

.. · ; · i ... ' i9 . · ; · ± is .. (6i ... ··37. 4 · ± · 2 . 4 .. i6i ... ... ···a:609ii'oi··· ... o.ss6

Table 3. Esti

mates of DEE based on the heart rate method from this study and three different models based on birds living in the trop ics, passeriformes, and passerines from a

rid and mesic climates.

All model estimates are based on the body weight of manakins (18.1 g).

method DEE (kJ day 1) animals

heart rate method 39. 1 GC manak ins

... ...

... ... ..

. ... .

Anderson

& J

etz [18) 38. 8 tropical birds

··· ... .

Nagy et

al.

[59]

745

passeriformes

Garamsz egi et

al.

79. 0 passerine from arid

[58) and mesic dimates

4 . Discussion

Despite their high-power-demancting courtship displays, heart rate measures of free-living birds indicate that GC manakins maintain a low DEE typical for tropical birds. Simi- lar results are obtained by several independent analytic approaches. Fin>t, using data collected via similar techni- ques, we found no difference between DEE of GC manakins and the spotted antbird, a non-lekking sympatric bird with a low DEE [49). Second, when we apply several models designed to provide general estimates of avian energy expenditure, manakins' energy expenditure is approximately 50% of what is expected [58,59]. Only the model by Anderson & Jetz [18] that is designed for tropical birds provides an estimate that is similar to what we obtained (table 3). In addition, compared with temperate-breeding birds, BMR is lower in tropical birds, inducting the GC mana- kin [12,43,44]. Our data confirm the measurements of BMR obtained by Vleck & Vleck [43] (21 kJ day 1) and Bartholomew et al. [44] (20 kJ day 1), which are both lower than what would be estimated by the equation of Asdtoff & Pohl [60]

for birds of the temperate zone (26 kJ day 1). Thus, our esti- mates of DEE as well as minimal MR together with previous estimates of BMR strongly suggest that GC manakins with their high-power-demancting courtship displays fall in the life history

strategy

classification of 'slow pare of life', typical for tropical birds [10,U,17,49).

We were surprised to discover that overall daily courtship activity in GC manakins lacks high joule costs. Although indi- vidual courtship displays per se have high power demands, reaching PMRs ninefold higher than minimal MR and three- fold higher than DEE [11,24), males actually spend minimal

time per day in courtship performance, using on average only 1.2% of their DEE on courtship dances. Short, highly intensive exercise bouts are thought to rely on anaerobic energy production, though there is good evidence that anaero- bic metabolism plays a limited role in fuelling manakin courtship. Fin>t, heart rates decrease the moment the courtship display ends [24], evidence that the birds have not accumulated an ~ deficit. Second, one can estimate aerobic scope, the maxi- mum ~ consumption of an animal during exercise over the animal's BMR, which for birds of 18.1 g such as manakins is estimated to be 36 [63). We find that manakins reach only about nine times BMR during courtship, whim is well within the animal's aerobic scope, hence courtship is likely to be sustained by aerobic metabolism. Thus, we are confident that our measures are valid estimates of energy expenditure during GC manakin courtship.

Collectively, males on a lek produce considerable noise in order to attract females and engage in courtship bouts in hopes of mating. Yet each individual invests minimal energy in the lekking enterprise. Even the most active male, perform- ing as many as 140 courtship displays per day, invests only 5%

of his DEE, because each display only lasts approximately 9.5 s.

This strategy of low joule costs could explain why manakins are able to display for up to seven months of the year and retain a lengthy life span.

In contrast to GC manakins, other lekking species engage in courtship involving high joule costs [5,62,64,65]. For example, sage grouse males perform on average 1680 displays during one or two bouts per day [ 63,66]. Thus, eadt bout of displaying (totalling about 1.4 h day 1) probably functions as an endur- anre exercise [6]. These energy demands may impose limits on the duration of the season of courtship, which lasts only one to two months.

Courtship enables females to assess male quality for purposes of mating [67). Presumably, males that engage in courtship that exacts high joule cost do so, at least partly, to demonstrate their capacity to withstand the behaviour- added energy demands [2 4]. The females might then choose males based on their behavioural and/or physio- logical adaptations to accumulate and efficiently metabolize the energy reserves required for courtship activity. Rather than being limited by total energy, male GC manakins appear to be performance-limited; that is, the courtship dis- play may be designed to demonstrate physiological and morphological qualities associated with their rapid, acrobatic performances. For example, male manakins have hypertro- phied skeletal muscles and fibres modified for increased

(7)

muscular contraction [68,69]. It is conceivable that the high heart rates achieved during courtship performance reflect optimal physiological function that females indirectly assess via these courtship displays.

Although there are many advantages to the use of heart rate telemetry for estimating metabolic rates of wild animals [35,39 42,49,51,62], there are also limitations. The signal detection range in dense forest placed limits on our ability to simultaneously observe some behaviours. For example, these studies would have benefitted from heart rate measures when focal males were interacting momentarily with other males or females. To overcome this difficulty, we combined heart rate data from one group of birds with estimates of activity from other birds. In addition, we were unable to stimulate peak heart rates during the calibration in captivity.

Thus, when predicting energy expenditure during courtship, we use values that are outside the calibration curve. Bishop &

Spivey [70] derived a model that estimated metabolic costs from heart-rate data that can be applied to our dataset:

body weight 18.1 g, heart weight 0.273 g, heart rate during dis- play 1017 beats min 1 and during 24 h 450 beats min 1. Plugging our data into the model, we find that GC manakins require 2.592 l of O2per day and 49 ml during the 5 min of courtship display, which corresponds to 1.9% of the daily budget, which is similar to the 1.2% estimated in our study [70]. Despite experimental limitations, we are confident that this work applying heart rate telemetry to study the energetics

of courtship behaviour provides valid estimates of the energetics of a tropical bird during its peak courtship period.

In conclusion, our data indicate that this tropical bird species with a lek mating system does not experience periods of courtship demanding high joule costs, as has been described for some temperate-breeding birds. Despite being present on the lek for much of the day, GC manakins show off their morphological and physiological proficiency in short display bouts across the day while maintaining a tropi- cal ‘slow pace of life’ metabolic strategy [32,67,71]. Many species of neotropical manakins engage in lekking behaviour, with a variety of patterns of timing and frequency of court- ship [72 74]. Additional research is required to determine whether the low joule cost strategy of GC manakins is common to other tropical lekking birds.

All procedures for animal use were approved by UCLA Chancellor’s Animal Research Committee, STRI and the Panamanian Autoridad Nacional del Ambiente.

Acknowledgements. We thank the Smithsonian Tropical Research Insti tute for support in Panama. The Princeton University 2008 tropical biology field course, G. Terranova and E. Choi contributed to data collection and processing. We thank J. Green for his advice on data analysis, and D. Altshuler and K. Nagy as well as anonymous referees for valuable comments.

Funding statement.This research was supported by NSF IBN 021319 to B.A.S.

References

1. Ho¨glund J, Alatalo RV. 1995Leks. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

2. Andersson M. 1994Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

3. Fisher RA. 1930The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

4. Zahavi A. 1975 Mate selection: a selection for a handicap.J. Theor. Biol.53, 205 – 214. (doi:10.

1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3)

5. Ho¨glund J, Ka˚la˚s JA, Fiske P. 1992 The costs of secondary sexual characters in the lekking great snipe (Gallinago media).Behav. Ecol.30, 309 – 315.

6. Vehrencamp SL, Bradbury JW, Gibson RM. 1989 The energetic cost of display in male sage grouse.Anim.

Behav.38, 885 – 896. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(89) 80120-4)

7. Vitousek MN, Rubenstein DR, Nelson KN, Wikelski M.

2008 Are hotshots always hot? A longitudinal study of hormones, behavior, and reproductive success in male marine iguanas.Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.157, 227 – 232. (doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.04.015) 8. Ricklefs RE. 1968 Patterns of growth in birds.

Ibis110, 419 – 451. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.

1968.tb00058.x)

9. Snow DW, Lill A. 1974 Longevity records for some neotropical land birds.Condor76, 262 – 267.

(doi:10.2307/1366339)

10. Ricklefs RE. 1976 Growth rates of birds in the humid New World tropics.Ibis118, 179 – 207. (doi:10.

1111/j.1474-919X.1976.tb03065.x)

11. Wiersma P, Chappell MA, Williams JB. 2007 Cold- and exercise-induced peak metabolic rates in tropical birds.Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA104, 10 866 – 10 871. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0707683104) 12. Wiersma P, Mun˜oz-Garcia A, Walker A, Williams JB.

2007 Tropical birds have a slow pace of life.Proc.

Natl Acad. Sci. USA104, 9340 – 9345. (doi:10.1073/

pnas.0702212104)

13. Williams JB, Miller RA, Harper JM, Wiersma P. 2010 Functional linkages for the pace of life, life-history, and environment in birds.Integr. Comp. Biol.50, 855 – 868. (doi:10.1093/icb/icq024)

14. Weathers WW. 1979 Climatic adaptation in avian standard metabolic rate.Oecologia42, 81 – 89.

15. Hails CJ. 1983 The metabolic rate of tropical birds.

Condor85, 61 – 65. (doi:10.2307/1367889) 16. Weathers WW. 1997 Energetics and thermoregulation

by small passerines of the humid, lowland tropics.

Auk114, 341 – 353. (doi:10.2307/4089237) 17. Wikelski M, Spinney L, Schelsky W, Scheuerlein A,

Gwinner E. 2003 Slow pace of life in tropical sedentary birds: a common-garden experiment on four stonechat populations from different latitudes.

Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B270, 2383 – 2388. (doi:10.

1098/rspb.2003.2500)

18. Anderson KJ, Jetz W. 2005 The broad-scale ecology of energy expenditure of endotherms.

Ecol. Lett.8, 310 – 318. (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.

2005.00723.x)

19. Chapman F. 1935 The courtship of Gould’s manakin (Manacus vitellinus vitellinus) on Barro Colorado

Island, Canal Zone.Bull. Am. Museum Nat. History 68, 471 – 525.

20. Schlinger BA, Day LB, Fusani L. 2008 Behavior, natural history and neuroendocrinology of a tropical bird.Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.157, 254 – 258.

(doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.05.015)

21. Fusani L, Schlinger B. 2012 Proximate and ultimate causes of male courtship behavior in golden- collared manakins.J. Ornithol.153, 119 – 124.

(doi:10.1007/s10336-011-0809-8)

22. Bostwick KS, Prum RO. 2003 High-speed video analysis of wing-snapping in two manakin clades (Pipridae: Aves).J. Exp. Biol.206, 3693 – 3706.

(doi:10.1242/jeb.00598)

23. Fusani L, Giordano M, Day LB, Schlinger BA. 2007 High-speed video analysis reveals individual variability in the courtship displays of male golden- collared manakins.Ethology113, 964 – 972. (doi:10.

1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01395.x)

24. Barske J, Schlinger B, Wikelski M, Fusani L.

2011 Female choice for male motor skills.

Proc. R. Soc. B278, 3523 – 3528. (doi:10.1098/

rspb.2011.0382)

25. Lasiewski RC. 1964 Body temperatures, heart and breathing rate, and evaporative water loss in hummingbirds.Physiol. Zool.37, 212 – 223.

26. Ju¨rgens KD, Fons R, Peters T, Sender S. 1996 Heart and respiratory rates and their significance for convective oxygen transport rates in the smallest mammal, the Etruscan shrewSuncus etruscus.

J. Exp. Biol.199, 2579 – 2584.

7

(8)

27. Skutch F. 1985 Clutch size, nesting success, and predation on nests of neotropical birds, reviewed.

Ornithol. Monogr.36, 575 – 594. (doi:10.2307/

40168306)

28. Karr JR, Nichols JD, Klimkiewicz MK, Brawn JD. 1990 Survival rates of birds of tropical and temperate forests: will the dogma survive?Am. Nat.136, 277 – 291. (doi:10.1086/285098)

29. Brawn JD, Karr JR, Nichols JD. 1995 Demography of birds in a neotropical forest? Effects of allometry, taxonomy, and ecology.Ecology76, 41 – 51.

(doi:10.2307/1940630)

30. Kleiber M. 1961The fire of life: an introduction to animal energetics. New York, NY: John Wiley.

31. Brody S. 1945Bioenergetics and growth. New York, NY: Hafner.

32. Clark CJ. 2012 The role of power versus energy in courtship: what is the ‘energetic cost’ of a courtship display?Anim. Behav.84, 269 – 277. (doi:10.1016/j.

anbehav.2012.04.012)

33. Taylor CR. 1974 Running in cheetahs, gazelles, and goats: energy cost and limb configuration.

Am. J. Physiol.227, 848 – 850.

34. Gill REet al. 2009 Extreme endurance flights by landbirds crossing the Pacific Ocean: ecological corridor rather than barrier?Proc. R. Soc. B276, 447 – 457. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1142) 35. Hawkes LAet al. 2011 The trans-Himalayan flights

of Bar-headed Geese (Anser indicus).Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA108, 9516 – 9519. (doi:10.1073/pnas.

1017295108)

36. Kotiaho JS. 2001 Costs of sexual traits: a mismatch between theoretical considerations and empirical evidence.Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc.76, 365 – 376. (doi:10.1017/S1464793101005711) 37. Speakman JR. 2000 The cost of living: field

metabolic rates of small mammals.Adv. Ecol. Res.

30, 177 – 297.

38. Piersma T. 2011 Why marathon migrants get away with high metabolic ceilings: towards an ecology of physiological restraint.J. Exp. biol.214, 295 – 302.

(doi:10.1242/jeb.046748)

39. Bevan RM, Woakes AJ, Butler PJ. 1994 The use of heart rate to estimate oxygen consumption of free-ranging balck-browed albatrossesDiomedea melanophrys.J. Exp. Biol.193, 119 – 137.

40. Bevan RM, Speakman JR, Butler PJ. 1995 Daily energy expenditure of tufted ducks: a comparison between indirect calorimetry, doubly labelled water and heart rate.Funct. Ecol.9, 40–47. (doi:10.2307/2390088) 41. Green J, Butler PJ, Woakes AJ, Boyd IL, Holder R. 2001

Heart rate and rate of oxygen consumption of exercising macaroni pinguins.J. Exp. Biol.204, 673–684.

42. Butler PJ, Green JA, Boyd IL, Speakman JR. 2004 Measuring metabolic rate in the field: the pros and cons of the doubly labelled water and heart rate methods.Funct. Ecol.18, 168 – 183. (doi:10.1111/j.

0269-8463.2004.00821.x)

43. Vleck CM, Vleck D. 1979 Metabolic rate in five tropical bird species.Condor81, 89 – 91.

(doi:10.2307/1367864)

44. Bartholomew GA, Vleck CM, Bucher TL. 1983 Energy metabolism and nocturnal hypothermia in two tropical passerine frugivores,Manacus vitellinusand Pipra mentalis.Physiol. Zool.56, 370 – 379.

45. Nagy KA. 2005 Field metabolic rate and body size.

J. Exp. Biol.208, 1621–1625. (doi:10.1242/jeb.01553) 46. Ricklefs RE, Konarzewski M, Daan S. 1996 The

relationship between basal metabolic rate and daily energy expenditure in birds and mammals.

Am. J. Physiol.147, 1047 – 1071.

47. Bisson I-A, Butler LK, Hayden TJ, Romero LM, Wikelski MC. 2009 No energetic cost of anthropogenic disturbance in a songbird.

Proc. R. Soc. B276, 961 – 969. (doi:10.1098/

rspb.2008.1277)

48. Bowlin MSet al. 2010 Grand challenges in migration biology.Comp. Gen. Pharmacol.50, 261 – 279.

49. Steiger SS, Kelley JP, Cochran WW, Wikelski M. 2009 Low metabolism and inactive lifestyle of a tropical rain forest bird investigated via heart-rate telemetry.

Physiol. Biochem. Zool.82, 580 – 589. (doi:10.1086/

605336)

50. Cochran WW, Wikelski M. 2005 Individual migratory tactics of New World Catharus thrushes: current knowledge and future tracking options from space.

InBirds of two worlds: the ecology and evolution of migration(eds P Marra, R. Greenberg), pp. 274 – 289. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

51. Sapir N, Wikelski M, McCue MD, Pinshow B, Nathan R. 2010 Flight modes in migrating European Bee- eaters: heart rate may indicate low metabolic rate during soaring and gliding.PLoS ONE5, e13956.

(doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013956)

52. Raim A. 1978 A radio transmitter attachment for small passerine birds.Bird Band.49, 326 – 332.

(doi:10.2307/4512391)

53. Fick A. 1870 Uber die Messung des Blutquantums in der Herzventrikeln.Sitz. Physik. Med. Ges.2, 16.

54. Green JA. 2011 The heart rate method for estimating metabolic rate: review and

recommendations.Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A158, 287 – 304. (doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.09.011) 55. Walsberg GE, Hoffman TCM. 2005 Direct calorimetry

reveals large errors in respirometric estimates of energy expenditure.J. Exp. Biol.208, 1035 – 1043.

(doi:10.1242/jeb.01477)

56. Bartholomew GA, Vleck D, Vleck CM. 1981 Instantaneous measurements of oxygen consumption during pre-flight warm-up and post- flight cooling in sphingid and saturniid moths.

J. Exp. Biol.90, 17 – 32.

57. Withers PC. 1977 Measurement of VO2, VCO2, and evaopartive water loss with a flow-through mask.

J. Appl. Physiol.42, 120 – 123.

58. Garamszegi LZ, Moreno J, Møller AP. 2006 Avian song complexity is associated with high field metabolic rate.Evol. Ecol. Res.8, 75 – 90.

59. Nagy KA, Girard IA, Brown TK. 1999 Energetics of free- ranging mammals, reptiles and.Annu. Rev. Nutr.19, 247–277. (doi:10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.247)

60. Aschoff J, Pohl H. 1970 Der Ruheumsatz von Vo¨geln als Funktion der Tageszeit und der Ko¨rpergro¨ße.

J. Ornithol.111, 38 – 47. (doi:10.1007/BF01668180) 61. Barron DG, Brawn JD, Weatherhead PJ. 2010 Meta- analysis of transmitter effects on avian behaviour and ecology.Methods Ecol. Evol.1, 180 – 187.

(doi:10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00013.x) 62. Kotiaho JS. 2000 Testing the assumptions of

conditional handicap theory: costs and condition dependence of a sexually selected trait.Behav.

Ecol. Sociobiol.48, 188 – 194. (doi:10.1007/

s002650000221)

63. Gibson RM, Bradbury JW. 1985 Sexual selection in lekking sage grouse: phenotypic correlates of male mating success.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.18, 117 – 123. (doi:10.1007/BF00299040)

64. Kotiaho JS, Alatalo RV, Mappes J, Nielsen MG, Parri S, Rivero A. 1998 Energetic costs of size and sexual signalling in a wolf spider.Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 2203 – 2209. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0560) 65. Thomas RJ. 2002 The costs of singing in

nightingales.Anim. Behav.63, 959 – 966.

(doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1969)

66. Wiley RH. 1973 The strut display of male sage grouse: a ‘fixed’ action pattern.Behaviour.47, 129 – 152. (doi:10.1163/156853973X00319) 67. Byers J, Hebets E, Podos J. 2010 Female mate

choice based upon male motor performance.Anim.

Behav.79, 771 – 778. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.

01.009)

68. Schultz JD, Hertel F, Bauch M, Schlinger BA. 2001 Adaptations for rapid and forceful contraction in wing muscles of the male golden-collared manakin:

sex and species comparisons.J. Comp. Physiol. A 187, 677 – 684. (doi:10.1007/s00359-001-0239-9) 69. Lowe RP. 1942 The anatomy of Gould’s manakin (Manacus vitellinus) in relation to its display.Ibis 84, 50 – 83. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1942.

tb03420.x)

70. Bishop CM, Spivey RJ. 2013 Integration of exercise response and allometric scaling in endotherms.

J. Theor. Biol.323, 11 – 19. (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.

2013.01.002)

71. Irschick DJ, Herrel A, Vanhooydonck B, Van Damme R. 2007 A functional approach to sexual selection.

Funct. Ecol.21, 621 – 626. (doi:10.1111/j.1365- 2435.2007.01281.x)

72. Bostwick KS. 2000 Display behaviors, mechanical sounds, and evolutionary relationships of the club- winged manakin (Machaeropterus deliciosus).Auk 117, 465 – 478. (doi:10.1642/0004-8038(2000) 117[0465:DBMSAE]2.0.CO;2)

73. DuVal EH. 2007 Adaptive advantages of cooperative courtship for subordinate male lance-tailed manakins.Am. Nat.169, 423 – 432. (doi:10.1086/

512137)

74. Shorey L. 2002 Mating success on white-bearded manakin (Manacus manacus) leks: male characteristics and relatedness.Behav. Ecol.

Sociobiol.52, 451 – 457. (doi:10.1007/s00265- 002-0540-9)

8

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

[r]

In this chapter, we present our empirical results that examine the average impact of instruction time on student performance (6.1), the impact of instruction time

down. It also secured discretion over the post-shutdown extension of contracts based on, among other things, com- panies’ cooperation in helping resolve the impasse with Khartoum.

I have chosen the novel Pride and Prejudice as my basis for the thesis as Austen has reflected the rules of the Regency society in her characters – Elizabeth Bennet and Mr Darcy.. I

In all species of the subtribe Nectemydina, one of these three variants occurs (see Appendix). In the parapatric or allopatric Trachemys scripta and T. ornata, two

accurately, and independently of the binary representation of the number of items. This paper is organized as follows: in Section 2 , a Pascal program for

In his introduction to the special issue, Seifudein Adem (2005: 441) rather noncommittantly summarizes Philips` contribution: “John Edward Philips claims that the teaching of

At 100 days of age, about half of the birds (the exact number of birds in each experiment is indi- cated in Figs 2 and 3) in each group were tested in