• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Mapping the aesthetic space of literature "from below"

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Mapping the aesthetic space of literature "from below""

Copied!
15
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Mapping the aesthetic space of literature “ from below ”

Christine A. Knoop

a,

*

,1

, Valentin Wagner

a,1

, Thomas Jacobsen

b

, Winfried Menninghaus

a

aMaxPlanckInstituteforEmpiricalAesthetics,Grüneburgweg14,60322Frankfurt,Germany

bHelmutSchmidtUniversity,UniversityoftheFederalArmedForcesHamburg,AllgemeineundBiologischePsychologie,Holstenhofweg85, 22043Hamburg,Germany

ARTICLE INFO Articlehistory:

Received28June2015

Receivedinrevisedform31January2016 Accepted3February2016

Availableonline1April2016 Keywords:

Aestheticappealdimensions Genre

Literaryform

Terminologicalmapping

ABSTRACT

Thepresentpaperaimstoelucidatetheconceptualstructureoftheaestheticsofliterature.

FollowingFechner’s“aestheticsfrombelow”(1876)andadoptingamethodintroducedby Jacobsen,Buchta,Kohler,andSchroeger(2004),weasked1544German-speakingresearch participantstolistadjectivesthattheyusetolabelaestheticdimensionsofliteraturein generalandofindividualliteraryformsandgenresinparticular (novels,shortstories, poems,plays,comedies).Accordingtoouranalysesoffrequency,meanlistrank,andthe CognitiveSalienceIndex,beautifulandsuspensefulrankhighestacrossalltargetcategories.

Forplays/comedies,funnyandsadturnedouttobethemostrelevantterms;fornovelsand shortstories,suspenseful,interestingandromantic;andforpoetryromantic,alongwiththe music-relatedtermsharmonious,rhythmic,andmelodious.Acomparisonofourresultswith analogousstudiesforvisualaestheticsandmusicyieldedacomprehensivemapofthe distribution of aesthetic appeal dimensions across sensory modalities and aesthetic domains,withpoetryandmusicshowingthegreatestoverlap.

ã2016TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierB.V.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCC BY-NC-NDlicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1.Introduction

Readingliterature,justlikeviewingvisualartworks(Leder,Belke,Oeberst,&Augustin,2004)orlisteningtomusic(Brattico, Bogert,&Jacobsen, 2013),involvesperceptual, cognitive,affective andevaluativeprocessing dimensions(Jacobs,2015).

Aestheticappreciationintegratesalltheseprocessingdimensionswithafocusonevaluatingtheperceivedaestheticappealofa workofliterature,andthefelthedonicrewardoftheexposure(cf.Kant’stheoryofaesthetic“judgment”and“liking”;Kant, 2007).Whencommunicatingtheperceivedaestheticappealofaworkofliteraturetoothers,weprojectourexperienceontothe spaceofavailableverbalconceptsandselectthosewefindmostappropriatetocapturethenatureofourimpression.The resultingattributionsofparticularaestheticappealdimensionstoparticularworksorgenresofliteraturereflecttheconceptual representations(1)ofouraestheticappreciationoftheseworksor(2)oftheexpectationswehaveofcertaingenres.These attributions,i.e.theselectiveuseofspecificverbalterms,allowforstraightforwardempiricalinvestigation.Theyalsorepresent validoptionstobeconsideredwhenselectingratingscaleitemsforempiricalresearchonliterature.

Dating backas faras the18thcentury, literaryhistory recordsa number ofattempts toestablishratingscalesfor evaluatingtheperceivedaestheticappealoftheworkofindividualauthors(see Spoerhase,2014,fora review).These historicaleffortshaveanumberofanalogsinrecentliteraryscholarshipthatattempt,inlightofhistorical,psychologicaland

*Correspondingauthor.Fax:+49698300479599.

E-mailaddress:christine.knoop@aesthetics.mpg.de(C.A.Knoop).

1ChristineA.KnoopandValentinWagnerarejointfirstauthors.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2016.02.001

0304-422X/ã2016TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierB.V.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense(http://creativecommons.org/

licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Poetics

j o u r n al h o m e p a g e : w w w . el s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / p o et i c

(2)

sociologicalfindingsandbasedontheoreticalreflection,toestablishmodelsofaestheticappreciationofliterature;such modelsseektoidentifydimensionsofevaluationthatareeithercharacteristicforindividualworksofartorthatrecurinthe historyofliteraryreception(cf.,e.g.,vonHeydebrand&Winko,1996),sometimeswithaspecificfocusonindividualgenres (cf.,e.g.,Ribeiro,2012).Intheempiricalandexperimentalresearchonartreception,anumberofscaleshavebeendeveloped tocapturespecificaspectsofaestheticexperienceinreadingliterature(see,forinstance,Green&Brock,2000;Koopman, 2013, 2015; Kuijpers, Hakemulder, Tan, & Doicaru, 2014; Kuiken, Campbell, & Sopcák,9 2012; Miall & Kuiken,1995).

Nevertheless,empiricalstudiesofliteraturehavenotyetsystematicallyinvestigatedtheverbalconceptsreadersusefor communicatingtheiraestheticperceptionsandevaluationsofliteratureandtherangeanddistributionoftheseconceptsas dependentonspecificgenresofliterature.

Foranaesthetics“frombelow”asproposedbyFechner(1876),itiscrucialtocollectdataonhowreadersthemselves verballyrepresenttheiraestheticexperiencesandexpectationsratherthantorelyonexpertevaluationsandexperimenter- selectedratingscalesonly.Importantly,however,thesedifferentapproachesarebynomeansmutuallyexclusive.Infact, previousresearchinmusicreception(Zentner,Grandjean,&Scherer,2008)hasshownthatacombinedbottom-upandtop- downapproachwasbestsuitedtocapturemusicalexperiences.Similarly,arecentstudyonfilmsrevealedthatparticipants facedwithexperimenter-selectedscalesgaveconsistentlyhighratingsforitemswhichonlyfewofthemhadmentioned unpromptedwhenaskedtoverbalizetheirresponsesintheirownterms(Wassiliwizky,Wagner,Jacobsen,&Menninghaus, 2015).Thus,aselectionofdimensionsofaestheticappreciationthatexclusivelyreliesonabottom-upretrievalprocessmay besubject to serious limitations. However, collectingspontaneous bottom-up conceptualizations of the aesthetics of literaturedoeshavepotentialnotonlyforselectingitemsforratingscalesinfuturestudies,butalsoforprovidingadeeper comparativeunderstandingoftheindividualdomainsofaestheticsthroughthelensoftheirrespectiveconceptualizations.

Similartootherartdomains,aestheticperceptionsandevaluationsofworksofliteraturetendtobeshapedby,andentail, comparisons topast experiences with particular literary forms or genres (see, e.g., Picon, 1953; Schmidt, 2007; von Heydebrand&Winko,1996; Weninger,1994; Zwaan,1994).Inturn,theexpectationsderivedfromthesecomparisons influencethechoiceoftermswithwhichreadersverballydescribetheiraestheticperceptionsandevaluationsofliterature.

Thesameholdsforexpectationsderivedfromknowledgereadershaveacquiredacademicallyorviaparatextorexcerpts(cf.

Dixon,Bortolussi&Sopcak,2015).Basedontheseassumptions,thegoalofthepresentpaperistoelucidatetheconceptual structureoftheaestheticsofliterature.Thisefforthasthepotentialtorevealbothform-andgenre-specificvariationsinthe relevantvocabulary and interdependenciesand similaritiesbetween genre-specific expressions.Our approach is well establishedasalanguage-basedwayofcollectingassociationstogaininsightintothementalrepresentationofconceptual structure (cf. Fehr & Russell, 1984; Kuehnast, Wagner, Wassiliwizky, Jacobsen, & Menninghaus, 2014). Moreover, by comparingourdatawiththosefromstudiesconductedinotheraestheticdomains(Augustin,Wagemansetal.,2012;Istók etal.,2009;Jacobsenetal.,2004),wewish(1)todeterminetermsthatareusedspecificallyforliteratureand(2)toidentify overlapsbetweenaestheticappealdimensionsmentionedforcertainliterarygenresandotherdomains,includingfashion, architecture,design,physicalattractiveness,andsoon(seeAugustin,Carbonetal.,2012,forasimilarapproachcomparing visualarts,film,andmusic).Thisapproachallowsforacomparativemappingofthedomain-andgenre-specificaesthetic expectationsofnon-expertsacrossthebroaderfieldofaestheticappreciation.

2.Capturingaestheticperceptionsandevaluations

Jacobsenetal.(2004)successfullyusedafreelistingtasktocollecttermsusedfordesignatingaestheticallyrelevant dimensionsofobjectsatlarge.Usingthesamemethodology,Istóketal.(2009)conductedastudyonmusic,whileAugustin, Wagemansetal.(2012)collectedtermsdescribingaestheticappealdimensionsregardingeightdifferentgroupsofvisual objects(buildings,cars,clothing,faces,interiordesigns,landscapes,geometricshapesandpatterns,andvisualart).

Ourstudyadoptsthebottom-up,exploratory“aestheticsfrombelow”approachusedinthesethreeprecedingstudies onotheraestheticdomains.Thisplacesusinapositionnotonlytoaddthemissingdataregardingliterature,butalsotobe thefirsttocomparetheavailabledataforthedifferentaestheticdomains.Literatureistraditionallycomprisedofthree majorforms:poetry,prose,anddrama(cf.Hegel,1975).Weincludedalloftheseformsinourstudy,yettreatedthem differentlyforavarietyofreasons.Ourgeneralsubsampleforplaysencompassedthedramaticformasawhole,butthe datawerefurtherspecifiedbytheinclusionofacomedies-onlysubsample.Forprose,wecollectedsubsampledatafortwo ofthemostpopularprosegenres:novelsandshortstories.Wedidnotaskfordimensionsofaestheticappealassociated withproseasageneralcategorybecause“prose”alsoencompassesscientificandessayisticwriting,whiletheinterestof ourstudyislimitedtothemorenarrowlydefinedfieldofliterature.Incontrast,weincludedpoetryexclusivelyasabroad category, without further generic subdistinctions. We did soanticipating that (1) unlike prose, “poetry” is a fairly meaningfulcategoryevenfornon-readersofpoeticgenres,and(2)termsdesignatingspecialgenresofpoetry(suchasthe elegy,theode,orthehymn)mayseemtoospecifictothebroaderpublic.Regardingplays,weintendedtocollectdatafor theall-inclusivecategory“play/drama”,whichincludesthepolargenres “tragedy” and“comedy”; however,duetoa communicationerrorbetweentheteamswhocollectedthedata,oneteamgathereddataforthegenreofcomedyonly.

(Fortunately,thedatawedidobtainatleastpartlycompensatedforthiserror,sincethedifferencebetweentheentriesfor thegeneralcategory“play/drama”andthoseforthesubgenre“comedy”largelyappeartoreflecttheroleoftragedy.See thediscussionsectionfordetails.)

(3)

3.Terminologicalmappingofliterature

3.1.Method 3.1.1.Participants

One thousand five hundred and forty-four students of different academic disciplines participated in our study (994womenand542men,8undisclosed);themeanagewas23.5years(SD=7.28,min=17,max=84).Studentswerechosen asrespondentsforthepragmaticreasonofavailability,butalsotokeepthedesignascomparableaspossibletothestudies conductedinotheraestheticdomains(Augustin,Wagemansetal.,2012;Istóketal.,2009;Jacobsenetal.,2004),whichwere alsobasedonstudentsamples.Oftheparticipants,875weretestedinHamburgand669inBerlin,withthesubsamplesizes varyingbetween134and423participants.2

3.1.2.Procedure

The data were collected in several lecture classes of different academic disciplines, including biology, cultural anthropology,economics,law,pedagogy,history,linguistics,literarystudies,andpsychology,toaddvariancetothesamples andreducegroup-specificeffects.3Toobtaintheaestheticperceptionandevaluationtermsusedfordifferentformsand genresofliterature,wecollecteddataforthesixsubsamplesLiterature(ingeneral),Poems,Novels,Shortstories,Plays(in general),andComedies.Thestudywasconductedasapaper-and-pencilsurvey;theparticipatingstudentsreceivedthe following instruction:“Pleasewritedownterms thatcouldbeusedtodescribetheaestheticsof literature.Pleaseuse adjectivesonly.Younowhave2minutes.”Dependingonthesubsample,theword“literature”wasreplacedby“poems”,

“novels”,“shortstories”,“plays(comediesortragedies)”or“comedies”.

Thewordingoftheinstructionwaschosenforthefollowingreasons.First,wewantedtoavoidtheintricaciesofcoding andevaluatingqualitativedataofafreeresponseformatandprofitfromthestraightforwardquantitativeanalysisofword frequencyandlistrank.Thereforetheinstructionruledoutfull-sentencedescriptionsofparticipants’conceptionsregarding aestheticallyrelevantfeaturesoftherespectivegenres.Atthesametime,theinstructioninvolvedthenotionthatfull-length accountsofaestheticexpectationsarelikelytoincludemultipledimensionsratherthanjustone.Therefore,whileextracting thetermsparticipantsconsiderrelevantfordesignatingaestheticappealdimensionsdoesnotamounttoanyconcrete descriptionofanaestheticexperience,itprovidesuswithalistofwordstobeusedaspartofsuchdescriptions.Moreover, thewordingofourinstructionwaskeptassimilaraspossibletotheinstructionusedinthestudiesonotheraesthetic domains(Augustin,Wagemansetal.,2012;Istóketal.,2009;Jacobsenetal.,2004)toensurethatwewouldbeableto compareourdatawiththeirsinameaningfulway.4

Ontopofcollectingthewordlistsweloggedtheparticipants’gender,age,andacademicdiscipline,andaskedthemto indicatewhethertheyconsideredthemselvesexpertsonliterature.Allparticipantsremainedentirelyanonymous.Thedata werecollectedinGerman;theresultsdisplayedinthefiguresand tablesinthisarticlearetranslationsoftheoriginal material(seeAppendixA,TableA1).

3.2.Results

3.2.1.Samplestatistics

Weprocessedandanalyzedthedatainseveralsteps.Inthefirststep,wecorrectedspellingmistakesandcomputedthe absoluteandmeannumbersofentries.Ourparticipantsgenerated9669answersintotal,correspondingto2131different words.Thetotalfigurealsoincludesillegibleentriesandtermslistedtwicebythesameparticipant.Thenumberofanswers perparticipantrangedbetween1and21(M=6.26;SD=3.76).Regardingthenumberofentries,aneffectofthespecificityof thereferencecategoriesbecameevident(F(5,1538)=11.49,p<.001):themeannumberofentriesforliteratureingeneral (4.95)wassmallerthanthoseforthemorespecificreferencecategories,i.e.,novels(7.18),poems(6.50),plays(6.71),and comedies(6.79).Moreover,thenumberofmeanentriesforshortstories(5.78)wassmallerthanfornovelsandcomedies (thesepost-hoccomparisonswerebasedonTukey’s“HonestlySignificantDifference”method).5

2ThestudywasconductedinBerlinandHamburgbecausetheparticipatingresearchershadtheirrespectiveinstitutionalaffiliationsinthesetwocities, andthuscouldenlistthehelpofbothresearchassistantsandlecturerswillingtoletusconductourstudyintheirclasses.

3Tosecurevarietyinoursamples,wepreselectedclassesfromdifferentresearchfields(naturalsciences,socialsciences,humanities)and,withinthese fields,differentdisciplines.Thefinalselectionofclasseswascontingentupontheagreementoftherespectivelecturerstoallocatetimeforourstudyduring class.

4Fromatheoreticalpointofview,itwouldhavebeenmoreprecisetoaskfortermsdesignating“perceivedaestheticappealdimensions”ratherthanjust

“aesthetics”.However,wechosethesimpleroptionbecauseweanticipatedthatthisproxywouldbeeasiertounderstandforourparticipants.

5Differencesofthe15comparisonswith95%CIandadjustedp-values:poems-literature=1.55[0.74;2.37],p<.001;novels-literature=2.23[1.12;3.34], p<.001;shortstories-literature=0.83[ 0.12;1.78],p=.130;plays-literature=1.76[0.73;2.79],p<.001;comedies-literature=1.84[0.97;2.71],p<.001;

novels-poems=0.68[ 0.37;1.72],p=.437;shortstories-poems= 0.73[- 1.60;0.15],p=.164;plays-poems=0.21[ 0.75;1.16],p=.990;comedies- poems=0.28[ 0.50; 1.07],p=.908; short stories-novels= 1.40 [ 2.56; 0.25], p=.007; plays-novels= 0.47 [ 1.68; 0.75], p=.880; comedies- novels= 0.39[ 1.48;0.69],p=.907;plays-shortstories=0.93[ 0.14;2.00],p=.128;comedies-shortstories=1.01[0.09;1.93],p=.022;comedies- plays=0.08[ 0.92;1.08],p=1.0.

(4)

3.2.2.Resultsfortheindividualsubsamples

Inthesecondstep,termsmentionedbyfewerthan5%oftheparticipantsintheindividualsubsampleswereexcludedfrom theanalyses(forasimilarcut-offprocedure,seeIstóketal.,2009;Jacobsenetal.,2004;vanGoozen&Frijda,1993).This procedurereducesvariabilitythatmightreflectidiosyncraticuses.Fortheremaining52terms(outoftheoriginal2131)we computed relative frequency (i.e., the number of participants mentioning a given term divided by subsample size), mean list rank (basedontherawlists,beforeexcludingterms),andtheCognitiveSalienceIndex(CSI;Sutrop,2001)foreachindividual subsample.TheCSIisthequotientofrelativefrequencyandmeanlistrankandisboundedbetween0and1,withhighervalues reflectingmoresalientterms.TheresultsfortheindividualsubsamplesaredepictedinFig.S1,withthetermsrankedforeach subsample.

Overall,beautifulandsuspensefulturnedouttobelistedmostfrequently(423and269;27.4%and17.4%,respectively).

Beautifulandboringaretheonlytermsmentionedineachsamplebyatleast5%;boringalsoturnedouttobetheonlytermof unambiguouslynegativevalence.Thetermssuspensefulandinterestingrankparticularlyhighlyinthenarrative(plot-based) genres;theseinclude dramatic plot,inaccordance withAristotle’sconceptofmythos (Aristotle,2005)as avariant of narrativeplot.

3.2.3.Comparingthesubsamples

Tofurtherexaminethecommonalitiesand differencesbetweenthesubsamples,wepreprocessed thedataslightly differentlyand retainedin all subsamplesall entriesthat werementioned byat least 10% of theparticipantsin one subsample.Thisleaves22terms,asopposedtothe52termsforthe5%cut-off.Thenewfrequencypatternswereexamined bycross-tabulation(usingthe

x

2-test)totestfordifferencesbetweenthesamples.Forall15comparisonsofthefrequency patterns,the

x

2-testrevealedsignificantdifferencesbetweenthesubsamples(p<.001,Bonferroni-correctedformultiple comparisons).Inthenextstep,wecalculatedtheoverlappingcoefficient(OVL;Inman&Bradley,1989;Marx,1976a,1976b) foreachpairofsubsamplesbyaddingupthelowerrelativefrequenciesinthetwodistributionsforeachofthementioned words.TheOVLranged between.32and.66, withthesmallestoverlapbetweenpoemsand comediesand thelargest betweennovelsandshortstories(seeTable1).WefurtheranalyzedtheOVLmatrixusingclassicalmultidimensionalscaling (MDS;alsocalledprincipalcoordinateanalysis;Gower,1966)andhierarchicalclusteranalysis(HCA,employingWard’s criterion).Thus,weidentifiedapatternofclustersinbothaclusteranalysisandanMDSbasedontheOVLmatrix(see Figs.S2AandB),withliteraturein generalandpoetryinparticularshowingvastsimilarities:theyarebothfrequently associatedwithtermssuchaspoetic,harmonic,rhythmical,andharmonious(seeFig.1).

Thesamplesfornovelsandshortstoriesshowsubstantialoverlap.Twoimportantexceptionsstandout:notsurprisingly, shortandsuccinctrankhighlyonlyintheshortstorysample,whereasromantic,thrilling,riveting,andexcitingarelistedfar morefrequentlywithreferencetonovels.Playsand comediesalsoshowaconsiderableoverlap;nevertheless,positive emotiontermsaremorefrequentinthecomedies-onlysample,whereasnegativeemotiontermsplayalargerroleinthe generalcategoryofplays,mostlikelyreflectingthetraditionalhegemonyofthetragedyoverthecomedy.

Termsusedtodenotedimensionsofaestheticappealcanbeexpectedtohaveanevaluativeconnotation(cf.Juslin,2013);

accordingly,mostofthetermswhichwerelistedbyourparticipantsareevaluativeinnature(Jacobsenetal.,2004,reportthe samefortheirdata).However,afewofthetermsobtainedinourstudyarelessobviouslyevaluative.Rhythmical,short,and sadinparticularatfirstappeartobenothingbutpurelydescriptiveattributes.Nevertheless,ratingsofsadnesshavebeen foundtocorrelatepositivelywithratingsforaestheticappreciationinseveralstudies(Hanich,Wagner,Shah,Jacobsen,&

Menninghaus,2014;Taruffi&Koelsch,2014);similarly,ratingsconfirmthatrhythmicallyregularpoemscorrelatepositively withaestheticliking(Obermeieretal.,2013).Andeventhetermshorthas,infact,atraditionasadistinctaestheticmerit terminrhetoric(seeSection3.3.4).Therefore,therearereasonstoassumethattheuseofseeminglypurelydescriptiveterms mayalso,inthecontextofaesthetics,includeanevaluativedimension.

Table1

Descriptivestatisticsofthesubsamples.

# Subsamples Location Numberof participants

Number of answers

Number ofterms

Mean answers per participant

Numberofterms listedbymore than5%ofthe subsample

Numberofterms listedbymore than10%ofthe subsample

1 2 3 4 5

1 Literature Berlin 274 1356 591 4.95 8 7

2 Poems Hamburg 423 2751 826 6.50 18 9 0.64

3 Novels Hamburg 134 962 411 7.18 23 11 0.63 0.42

4 Short stories

Berlin 223 1288 570 5.78 16 10 0.54 0.34 0.66

5 Plays Berlin 172 1154 505 6.71 17 11 0.42 0.36 0.58 0.50

6 Comedies Hamburg 318 2158 791 6.79 16 10 0.50 0.32 0.60 0.57 0.58

overall 1544 9669 2131 6.26

(5)

ironic succinct sad witty dramatic humorous short tragic entertaining exciting funny thrilling rhythmic melodious harmonious riveting boring romantic interesting poetic suspenseful beautiful

0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5

Literature Poems Novels Short stories Plays Comedies

Fig.1.Relativefrequenciesplottedfortermsmentionedbyatleast10%oftheparticipantsinonesubsample,orderedbyfrequencyintheliterature subsample.Thegreyscaleindicateswhetherthetermwasmentioned/listedbylessthan5%(lightgrey),5%to10%(grey),ormorethan10%(darkgrey)ofthe participantsofeachindividualsample.

1.2 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0

HCA (based on Jaccard Dissimilarity)

harmonious melodious rhythmic poetic beautiful romantic entertaining funny sad riveting thrilling exciting boring interesting suspenseful humorous ironic witty dramatic tragic short succinct

Fig.2.DendrogramoftheHierarchicalClusterAnalysisforthe22adjectives.

(6)

3.2.4.Overallmapping

Inordertointegrateallgenre-specificfindingsintoasemantic“map”ofthefieldofliteraryaesthetics,wecalculateda dissimilaritymatrixbasedontheco-occurrenceofthetermsonthelistsofallparticipantsandanalyzedthismatrixusing HCAandMDS.AsadissimilaritymeasureweusedtheJaccardIndex(Real&Vargas,1996),whichtakesintoaccountonly positivematchesandnon-matches.

Threedistinctclustersemergedfromouranalysis:onecomprisingthetermbeautifulalongwithsound-andprosody- relatedterms(e.g.,melodious,rhythmical),asecondcomprisingplot-andemotion-relatedterms(e.g.,suspenseful,thrilling, sad), andathirdthat seemedtobemoreheterogeneousatfirst,butthatcouldinasecondstepbedividedintothree subclustersthatappeartoberelatedtothespecificnaturesofshortstories,comedies,andplaysingeneral(seeFig.2).

ThedissimilaritymatrixwasalsofedintoaclassicalMDSprocedure;Fig.3depictsthetwo-dimensionalMDSsolution.6 Furthermore,basedontheMDScoordinatesofthetermsandtheirfrequenciesinthesamples,wecalculatedpointsthat representthelocalizationofthesubsamplesinthistwo-dimensionalaestheticappreciationspaceandplottedthemas vectorarrowsintotheMDSplot.Thefirst(horizontal)dimensionshowsaclearbipolarstructurewithtermsdesignating

“poetic”qualitiesandmusicalityontheoneside(withtheterm“poetic”linguisticallynotbeingrestrictedtotheverbal featuresofpoetrybutalsoincluding“poetic”feelings,atmospheres,moods,andrelatedphenomenainabroadersensethat extends to other art forms) and narrative and conceptual structure on the other. The second (vertical) dimension distinguishesbetween terms referringto emotional and potentially immersivestates at one end and humorous and intellectualstatesattheother.

−0.4 −0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4

−0.4

−0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4

literature

poems novels

short stories plays

comedies

beautiful

boring

entertaining

exciting

harmonious

humorous

interesting

ironic

melodious

poetic rhythmic

riveting

romantic sad

short

succinct

suspenseful

thrillingtragic

witty

dramatic

funny

Fig.3.Two-dimensionalMultidimensionalScalingsolution.Thefiveclustersarecodedbycolor,thelocationofthesixsubsamplesinthisspaceisindicated bytheblackvectors.

6 TheGoodness-of-Fit(GOF)forthetwo-dimensionalsolutionisonly.17,andthusremainsbelowconventionalcriteria.YetinthespecificMDS-procedure weapplied,addingfurtherdimensionswhileincreasingtheGOFleftthefirsttwodimensionsunchanged(cf.Gower,1966).Furthermore,onlythefirst twodimensions,whichbydefinitioncapturemorevariancethanthesubsequentones,appeartoreflectmoregeneralaspectsoftheaestheticspaceof literature,whereastheotherdimensionsshowastrongerfocusonmoreparticularaspects.Inanyevent,thetwo-dimensionalsolutionpresentedinFig.3 capturesonlyalimitedamountofthevarianceincludedinourdata.

(7)

3.3.Discussion

Theresultsofthisstudyhighlightthevarianceandcomplexityofhowtheperceivedaestheticappealdimensionsof literaturearemappedontoverbalconceptsbyreaders.Manyofthetermslisteddidnotmakeitpastthecut-offprocedures describedabove;byimplication,thetermsthatpassedthecut-offprocedurebynomeansrepresenttheentirevariancein aestheticappealdimensionsassociatedwiththecategoriesunderscrutiny.Nonetheless,thetermsthatendedupbeing namedbyatleast10%(22)or5%(52)oftheparticipantsinatleastonesamplestillofferasubstantial–and,aswillbecome evidentinstudy2,highlydistinctive–rangeofaestheticappealdimensionsregardingliterature.Inthefollowing,wewill individuallydiscussthemosthigh-rankingterms.

3.3.1.Beautiful

Thetermbeautiful–beinglistedmostfrequentlyoverall–provesitspreeminenceinaestheticsinthefieldofliteratureno lessthaninotherdomainsthathavebeenstudied,despitenumerousclaimsmadesincethe18thcenturythatliteratureno longerprimarilyaimsatbeauty(cf.Jauss, 1991;Lessing,1984).Tobesure,theGermanwordschönhasaconsiderablybroader application and higherusagefrequency thantheEnglish termbeautiful(Wortschatzprojekt UniversitätLeipzig, 2015).

However,contrarytoanindiscriminatebiastowardbeauty,ourdatarevealdifferencesregardingtherole oftheterm beautifulinthedifferentaestheticdomains,withthepatternsofco-occurrenceprovidingmorespecificinsightintowhat readersmightmeanwhenlabelingliterarytextsasbeautiful(seeFig.3):thetermclustersmoststronglywithromantic, followedbypoetic,rhythmic,melodious,andharmonious.Apparently,theattributionofbeautytoaliterarytextreliesmore stronglyontheprosodicqualitiesoflanguageandon“romantic”or“poetic”feelings,moods,andatmospheresthanon narrativeplotinthestrictersense.Inlinewiththispatternofco-occurrence,beautyseemstobepreeminentlyassociated withqualitiesofpoetry.Themorecolloquialmeaningsofpoetic,i.e.,soulfulandenchanting,hintattheaffectivequalitiesof thetermbeautiful.Romanticistheonlyoneoftheclusteringtermsthatcouldbereadasreferringtocertaincontentfeatures (typicaloftheRomanticperiod),butitmayequallypertaintoacharacteristicallyRomanticform,aswellas(intheeveryday senseoftheword)topowerfullysentimental,evocative,oratmosphericeffectsthatcouldbeduetobothformandcontent.

3.3.2.Suspenseful

Thetermsuspensefulisthesecondmostfrequentlymentionedtermoverall;itshighestscoresoccurintheplot-based samples.Thefactthatmostoftheformsandgenresweexaminedareplot-basedmaythereforeserveasanexplanationforthe term’soverallhighsalience.Inthesamplesfornovels,shortstories,andcomedies,suspensefulislistedmoreoftenthan beautiful.

Narrative suspensemakes readersfear ordesireparticular plotoutcomes,which canvaryin likelihood duringthe differentstagesofanarrative;thiscreatesaneedforresolutionthataccompaniestheentiresuspensefultrajectory(cf.Anz, 1998;Berlyne,1960;Carroll,1996;Fill,2007;Lehne&Koelsch,2015;Löker,1976;Wulff,1996;Zillmann,1980).Importantly, thistensefeedbackloopbetweenchangingdegreesofuncertaintyandanticipationofthepotentialresolutionstrongly dependsonthewaythecontentisnarrativelyarranged.Infact,artisticnarrativearrangementscanevenbuildsuspense whenreadersalreadyknowtheoutcomeoftheplot(cf.Gerrig, 1989;Hoeken&vanVilet,2000;Lehne&Koelsch,2015;Yanal, 1996).Suspensealsoservestoincreasethegeneralemotionalsusceptibilityoftheaudience(cf.Oatley, 1999;Vorderer,Wulff,

&Friedrichsen,1996).Consistentwiththesetheoreticalassumptions,thetermsuspense clustersstronglywithemotion termssuchasthrillingandrivetinginoursample.

3.3.3.Interesting

Theterminterestingisthefourthhighestscoringterminourliterature,novel,andshortstorysubsamples.Intheoretical aesthetics,theattribute“interesting”hasbeenacknowledgedasanessentialcategorysincethe18thcentury(cf.Diderot, 1995; Garve, 1974; Ostermann,1997; Sulzer,1967). Friedrich Schlegel in particular developed a concept of aesthetic evaluationinwhichtheinteresting,notthebeautiful,isthemainreferencepoint(Schlegel,2001).Morerecentempirical researchsupportsthenotionoftheinterestingasaprimecategoryofaestheticappreciation(Silvia,2005,2010).

Ourpoetrysubsamplescoresverylowonthedimensioninteresting,whichmaybeduetovariousreasons.Firstly,since the18thcenturytheinterestinghasbeenconsideredtoappealtotheintellectratherthantheheart,whichiswhyitwas primarilyappliedtonovelsandplays;poetry,bycontrast,longcontinuedtobelargelyconceivedasfocusingonhighly personalfeelingsandonbeautifulpoeticdiction(Hegel,1975;Lukács,1971;Schlegel,2001).Secondly,inaccordancewith Schlegel’s(2001)theoreticalreflections,Silvia(2010)suggeststhatahighdegreeofnoveltyisanimportantfactorforfinding somethinginteresting.However,thepoetrywithwhichaveragereaderstypicallycomeintocontact(inschool,atuniversity, inprintedanthologies,ongreetingcards,atweddingorbirthdaycelebrations)isoftenrepresentativeoftraditionallyrical forms and motifs; contactwithlyrical texts thata contemporary readerwould deem innovative,and therefore more interesting,israrebycomparison(cf.Shetley,1993).

3.3.4.Otherterms

Overall,wefoundahighnumberofemotion-relatedterms.Needlesstosay,emotiongenerallyplaysanenormousrolein theaestheticexperiencewithandtheaestheticevaluationofliterature.Theparticularlyhighscoresofemotiontermsinthe

(8)

listsforplaysingeneralandcomediesinparticularconfirmthenotionthatdramaasagenreaimsprimarilyatevokingstrong affectiveresponsesinaudiences(Aristotle,2005;Zillmann,1980,1983).

Twofrequentlyusedqualityjudgmentsregardingworksofliterature,namely,attributionsofgoodnessorbadness(“a goodnovel”,a“badplay”),areentirelylackingfromourdata.Attributionsoftheadjectivesgoodandbadreflectaparticularly abstractandunspecifictypeofaestheticjudgmentthatfailstoprovidehintsregardingeithertextualfeaturesorexperiential responsedimensions;theexplicitinstructiontolistaesthetics-specificadjectivesmayhavepreventedourparticipantsfrom listingthissimpledichotomy.Theabsenceofthesetermsmaythuswellbeataskeffect;confirmingthisassumption,the terms“good”and“bad”werelikewisenotlistedinthestudiesonotheraestheticdomainsthatusedsimilarinstructions (Augustin,Wagemansetal.,2012;Istóketal.,2009;Jacobsenetal.,2004).Similarly,thetermpleasantdoesnotappearinour results,despitebeingoneofthemorefrequentlyemployeditemsinaestheticratingscales.Thismight,again,beduetothe relativelyunspecificnatureoftheterm,designatingasitdoesabroadrangeofpositiveexperiencesthatcouldbemediated byagreatvarietyofemotionsandotherresponsedimensions,aswellasaestheticallyrelevanttextfeatures.

Theanalysisof theterms listedfor poetryreveals a strongbiasfor music-related terms, mostnotablyrhythmical, melodious,andharmonious.Theseresultscoincidewithalong-standingtraditionofequatingpoetswithsingersandpoems withsongs(e.g.,Hegel,1975;Herder,1998;Nietzsche,1993),whichbecameacornerstoneoftheRomanticconceptofpoetry andhasshapedprototypicalnotionsofpoetryeversince.Ourbottom-updatademonstratetheenduranceofthisconceptfar beyondascholarlyawarenessoftheunderlyingintellectualtraditions.Atthesametime,termsdesignatingnarrativeand conceptualproperties,suchasthrilling,riveting, andsuspenseful,which prove very importantfor thenovel,are rarely mentionedinthepoetrysubsample;thissupportsthenotionthatbothformalandcontent-relatedcharacteristicsofliterary genresclusterwithspecificaestheticevaluations(cf.Ribeiro,2012).

Asweexpected,thedataobtainedfornovelsandshortstoriesclusterclosely.Yetwealsofoundsomedifferencesthatare worthacloserlook.First,thefrequenciesfortheadjectiveshortsetthegenresneatlyapart.Onemightdismissthisasan indicationthatourparticipantsconfoundedthenameofthegenrewithitspotentialaestheticmerits;however,shortness (gr.brachytes,lat.brevitas)isinfactawell-establishedcategorydesignatingtherhetoricalachievementofcondensinga messageandmakingitunusuallycompact(Quintilian,1953;foranempiricalstudyoneffectsofrhetoricalbrevityinthe processingofproverbs,cf.Menninghausetal.,2015).Infact,ashortstorythatisnotshortwouldviolategenreexpectations, includingaestheticrewardexpectations.Thus,thehighscoresforshortmaynot,afterall,beameretaskeffect.

Second,thetermsromantic,thrilling,riveting,andexcitingwerelistedveryfrequentlyfornovelsbutdonotseemtobe expectedpropertiesofshortstories—whichmightbafflereadersofPoe,Maupassant,orChekhov.Onepossibleexplanation forthiscouldbefoundintheGermancurricula:theGermanshortstoryafter1945isfeaturedprominentlyinGerman secondaryschools;therefore,mostorallofourparticipantswillhavebeenexposednotonlytoexamplesofthegenreitself, butalsotoacatalogofitsspecificfeatures:thistypeofshortstorycommitstostraightforward,openstorylineswhile forgoingcomplicatedchainsofcauseandeffect,highlyemotionalsuspense–resolutionsequences,andevenunambiguous endings(Weyrauch,1989).Knowledgeofthisparticulartraditionandresultinggenreexpectationsmightbereflectedinthe lowerratingsforexciting,thrilling,andrivetinginourshortstorysubsamplecomparedtothenovelsubsample.

3.3.5.ThetwodimensionsoftheMDSsolution

Termsreferringtopoeticqualitiesofatextandtermsreferringtoitsnarrativeand/or thematicstructureseemto form the opposite ends of the horizontal axis of our MDS solution (see Fig. 3). These poles are most markedly representedbythetermsbeautiful,whichrefersmostlytothepoeticandformalappealoftexts,andsuspenseful,which primarilyreferstoplottrajectories.However,thisresultshouldnotbereadassupportingaform-contentdissociation.In fact,theplotsideofthespectrumactuallyrepresentsnotjustcontentalone,butaparticulartypeofinteractionbetween formand content.

TheverticalaxisofourMDSgraph(seeFig.3)showstermsthatrefertomoreintellectualaspectsofreading(witty, humorous,ironic)ontheonehand,andterms thatpertaintothetext’semotional contentortheaffectiveresponsesit mayevoke(e.g.,suspenseful, sad, boring)ontheother (fora basis forthis distinction betweenemotions perceivedas beingrepresentedbyaworkofartandtheactualfeltemotionsoftheaudience,cf.Juslin,2013;seealsoRegel,Gunter,&

Friederici,2011;Nagelsetal.,2013).Notably,inthis graphcomediesandplaysingeneral (whichbydefinitioninclude comedies)areclearlyseparated:comparedtocomedies,playsingeneral arealotfarther removedfromthepolethat entailswitty,humorous,andironic,and farclosertothepolethatentailssuspenseful,sad,boring.Wesurmise thatthe differencesbetweentheresponsesgivenforcomediesonlyandforplaysingeneralprimarilyreflecttheroleoftragedy.

Inaccordance withthis interpretation,thecategoryof playsingeneral includesthetermtragic,whichis listedmuch morefrequently than humorous and witty(although less frequently thanfunny), and also thetermsad,which is the highest-scoringtermintheentiresubsample. Inalllikelihood, separatedatafor tragedywould havefurtherenlarged this distance betweenplaysin general and comedies, because theinclusion of comedies in thecategory of plays in generalshouldkeepthisdistanceatamoremoderatelevel.Thus,thelackofaseparatedatasetfortragediesisatleast partly compensated by the differences between our data for comedies and for plays in general. In light of this interpretationofthedata,itappearsthatfromanaffectiveperspective,thetwodramaticgenresofcomedyandtragedy areperceivedtohavetwoentirelydifferentaffectivesignatures,notsimplyinvalence(positive negative)andaffective content(funny sad),butalsoregardingthequalityof theevokedaffects andtheextenttowhich theyarelinkedto cognitive processes(cf.,e.g., Taylor,1988; Morreall,1983).

(9)

Whilethetermsonthemoreintellectualendofthespectrumhaveaprimarysemanticfocusonappealdimensions ascribedtothetext(e.g.,witty,humorous,ironic),theaffectiveendofthespectrumentailsalargenumberoftermsthatplace aprimaryfocusonthetext’s(potential)effectonthereader(e.g.,suspenseful,exciting,thrilling).

4.Comparingliteraturewithotherdomains

4.1.Studiesanddata

Inordertocompareourresultsregardingthedomainofliteraturewithother(aesthetic)domains,weconductedanalyses includingtheresultsofourstudyandtheresultspublishedbyJacobsenetal.(2004)onaestheticobjectsatlarge,Istóketal.

(2009)onmusic,andAugustin,Wagemansetal.(2012)oneightdifferentclassesofvisualobjects:buildings,cars,clothing, faces,interiordesign,landscapes,geometricshapesandpatterns,andvisualart.Althoughthestudiesallemployedafree listingmethod,there areseveral differencesbetweenthem:theydifferedregarding thelanguagein whichtheywere conducted(German,Finnish,orDutch),restrictionsontheanswerformat(onlyadjectivesvs.unconstrained),andthetime accordedforthetask(2min,5min,orunconstrained);foranoverview,seeTable2.

Thetermsmentioned byatleast5%in therespective samples,along withtheirfrequencies,werethebasis forthe followinganalyses(thesedataareavailableaspartofthepublishedstudies;forourdata,seeSection3.2.2).Basedonthese frequencydata,wecalculatedtheOVLforeachsamplepairing(seeTableS1),which wefedintoanHCAand anMDS procedure.

4.2.Results

Unsurprisingly, individualsamples tend toshow thegreatestoverlapwithothersamples fromthesame domain;

however,weidentifiedafewinterestingexceptions.Literatureingeneralprimarilyintersectswithnovelsandpoems,but whencomparedtononliterarydomains,itshowsthegreatestoverlapwithvisualarts.However,theliterarysubdomainof poems–again,whencomparedtononliterarydomains–showsthegreatestoverlapwithmusic,followedbythevisual artsandvisualaestheticobjectsingeneral.Thetermsmentionedformusichavethegreatestoverlapwiththoselistedfor poetry, followedbyvisualart.Visualart,inturn,showedthegreatestoverlapwithclothing,followedbyliteraturein general, andthen visualaesthetic objects in general,faces, andcars. Hence,our dataalso reveal someconsiderable similaritiesbetweenthetermsusedtoconceptualizetheaestheticappealdimensionsofvisualart,music,poetry,and literatureingeneral.

Moreover,wetransformedtheOVLmatrixintoadissimilaritiesmatrixandfeditintoanHCAprocedure,usingWard’s method.Theresultshowstwoclearlyseparateclusters,oneofwhichcomprisesmostsamplesofthevisualdomain.The otherclusterincludestheliterarysamples,aswellasthemusicandlandscapesamples.Bothclusterscanbefurtherdivided intotwosubclusters:withinthevisualcluster,manufacturedartifactsaregroupedtogether,andsoarefaces,geometrical Table2

Overviewofthecomparisonbetweenstudies.

Study Year Objectclass N Language Answer

format

Design Procedure Time

Jacobsenetal.(2004) 2004 Objects 311 German Adjectives Paper&

pencil

2min

Istóketal.(2009) 2009 Music 300 Finnish Adjectives Paper&

pencil

5min Augustin,Wagemansetal.

(2012)

2012 Buildings 178 Dutch No

restriction

Within participant

Online No

restriction

Cars 177

Clothing 175

Faces 175

Geometricshapes&

patterns

173

Interiordesign 175

Landscapes 177

Visualart 177

Ourstudy Literature 274 German Adjectives Between

participant

Paper&

pencil

2min

Poems 423

Novels 134

Shortstories 223

Plays(tragediesor comedies)

172

Comedies 318

(10)

patterns,andvisualaestheticobjects.Withintheliterarycluster,onesubclustercomprisesthenarrativegenres,andthe othercomprisesliteratureingeneral,poems,music,andlandscapes(Fig.4).

AnMDSbasedonthedissimilaritiesmatrixlargelymirrorstheresultoftheclusteranalysis(seeFig.5).7Again,wefinda clustercomprisedoftheplot-basedliterarygenres,andonecomprisingliteratureingeneral,poems,andmusic,thelastthree clusteringsomewhatmoreloosely.Thereareslightdifferencesinthetwonon-literature-relatedclusters:onecomprises manufacturedartifactsofpracticaluse,includinggeometricalpatterns,butnotthevisualarts;theothercontainsthevisual arts,landscapes,faces,andvisualaestheticobjectsingeneral.AninterpretationofthetwodimensionsofthisMDSisnot straightforward.

4.3.Discussion

Aparticularlyinterestingresultofthecomparisonofourdatawiththoseofpreviousstudiesisthestrongoverlapbetween thetermsusedformusicandpoetry;thisunderscoresourfindingthatthephonological(includingprosodic)propertiesof poetry,alongwithitsemotionalaspects,aredeemeditsmostdistinctivecharacteristics.Moreover,thecontentofpoetry– consisting,asittypicallydoes,ofsituationalminiaturesanddepictionsofmomentarymoodstates(cf.Schlaffer,2012)– usuallydoesnotinvolveanythingresemblingafull-blownnarrativeplot.Asaresult,poetryappearstofavoracomparison withmusicoveracomparisonwithotherliterarygenresthatarebasedonplot.Ofcourse,anumberofsubgenresmight renderthisclear-cutsolutionsomewhatcomplicated:thenarrativelystructuredballad,themeteredandsometimeseven rhymedtragedy,freeverse.However,whileourdataforpoetrydonotcovertheseandothersubgenres,theydoreflectnot merelytheexpectationsofourparticipants,butalsoimportant–ifnotpredominant–featuresofpoetry.

1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0

visual arts clothing interior design cars

buildings patte rns objects faces plays comedies novels short sto ries literature poems music landscapes

Fig.4. DendrogramoftheHierarchicalClusterAnalysisforthe16objectclassesinthecomparedstudies.

7 Thetwo-dimensionalMDSsolutiononlyyieldedaGOFof.41.However,aswasthecaseforthefirstMDSreportedinthispaper,thepatternfoundforthe twofirstdimensionsdidnotchangeasweaddedmoredimensionstotheMDSsolution.WeherereportthisMDSsolutiononlyasanadditionalvisualization ofthedataunderlyingtheclusteranalysis.

(11)

Generally, theartisticdomains (literature,visual arts,music)sharemany ofthe termsused toconceptualizetheir aestheticappeal;thiscorrespondstothewayinwhich18thcenturyaestheticscomprisedallarts,whichuntilthenhadbeen consideredexclusivelyasindividualartes,underthenewall-encompassingsingularofart(cf.vonSchelling, 1989).Moreover, ourfindingthatliteratureingeneral,whencomparedtononliterarydomains,hasthegreatestoverlapwithvisualartscanbe readassupportingthelong-standingutpicturapoesistradition(cf.Horace,2011;Lessing,1984).

Insomerespects,however,wefoundcleardomain-baseddifferences:ugly,forinstance,islistedonlyforvisualobjects andmusic.Itappearsthatuglinessinliterature,ifitcanbefoundatall,isamatterofsemanticcontentratherthanaesthetics (Eco,2007).Theclearestantonymtobeautifulintheliteraturesampleisthetermboring(cf.Lorand,1994)–whichinturnis barelyrelevantfortheotherdomains.

5.Generaldiscussion

5.1.Keyresults

(1) Inallstudiestodate(includingours)thatanalyzethelinguistictermsusedtodesignateexpectedorperceived aestheticappealdimensions,theconceptofbeautyhasbeenfoundtobeprevalentinalltesteddomains.Atthesametime, beautifulisbynomeansthemostfrequentlymentionedtermineachofthedomainsinourstudy.Itscoredhighestinthe subsamplesfor poetryandliterature ingeneral;yetintheplot-basedsubsamples,othertermsturnedouttobemore significant:suspensefulinthecaseofthenovelaswellastheshortstory,funnyandsadinthesubsampleofplaysingeneral, andsuspenseful,funny,witty,andironicinthecomedysubsample.Nevertheless,countingalltermslistedintheentirestudy, beautifulismentionedmostfrequentlyoverall.Regardingonlythecategoriesofliterature,whichwereinvestigatedinthe presentstudy,attributionsofthetermbeautifulclusterstronglywithtermsreferringtothe“poetic”qualitiesofdictionand emotionaltonality,andfarlesswithtermsthataremorecloselyassociatedwithplottrajectories.Overall,theselectionof verbalconceptswegatheredrevealsthat,inmanyinstances,conceptswithalong-standingtraditioninaesthetictheoryplay animportantroleinnon-expertconceptualizationsoftheaestheticsofliterature;thisappearstoapplyfarbeyondany scholarlyawarenessofthistradition.

−0.4 −0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4

−0.4

−0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4

literature

poems novels

short sto ries plays comedies

music

objects visual a rts landscapes

faces patterns

cars clothing interior design

buildings

Fig.5.Two-dimensionalMultidimensionalScalingsolutionforthe16objectclassesofthecomparedstudies.

(12)

(2)Thecomparisonofpreviousstudieswithourownfindingsunderscoresthecommonalitiesbetweenmusicandpoetry thatcanalreadybeextractedfromourowndataalone.Theconsistentdatapatternconfirmsthatthesecommonalitiesare notonlyseenbyexpertsfamiliarwiththelongstanding“Romantic”traditionoftheorizingpoetry,butareseeminglyalsopart ofthewayreadersconceiveofpoetrytothisveryday.

(3)Regardingtheaestheticsofliterature,ourdatashowtwopreeminentfactors:plot-basedappealdimensions,which areprimarilyimportantfornovels,shortstories,andplays,and“poetic”appealdimensions,whicharemostlybasedon prosody-andmusic-relatedaspectsaswellasonspecialemotionaltonalities.Thissecondgroupofappealdimensionsisby nomeansexclusivelyrelatedtopoetry(whereitisparticularlystrong),butextendsintothebroadersampleofliteraturein general.

(4) Our approach “frombelow” identifies verbalconcepts which, together, forma conceptual map of prototypical aestheticperceptionsandevaluationsregardingparticularliterarygenres.Theparticipantsinourstudydidnotevaluate individual works of literature, but listed expected dimensions of aesthetic appreciation regarding literary genres;

furthermore,theychosetheirownwordingratherthanrespondingtoexpert-selecteditems.Nodataofthistypehasbeen collectedinpreviousempiricalresearchonliterature.

5.2.Limitations

First,itisimportanttonotethattheappealdimensionsextractedinourstudyrelyonabstractrepresentationsofgenre concepts.Inalllikelihood,theseabstractconceptsareidealizedentitiesthatdonotcoverthefullhistoricalvarianceofthe concreteworksofliteraturefoundacrossmultiplelanguagesandcultures.Moreover,ourdataexclusivelyreflectappeal dimensionsidentifiedbyasampleoftoday’sreadersandarelikelytobesubjecttoongoingchange.Therefore,therangeof dimensionsofappealidentifiedheremustbeconsideredasafirst,rathergeneralguidepost.

Second,thefourstudiesontheconceptualstructureofspecificaestheticdomainsthatwecomparedwereconductedin differentlanguages:German,Finnish,andDutch(seeTable2).Alldata,includingourown,havebeentranslatedintoEnglish.

Therefore,anycomparisonmustbearinmindlanguage-specificdifferencesinusagefrequency,emotionalvalence,habitual connotations,and,notleast,semanticsbetweentheselinguisticgroups(forotherproceduraldifferencesseeTable2).

Third,collectingfreelistingdatabydefinitiondoesnotprovideanyinformationabouttheparticipants’previousgenre exposureandpreferences,thoughthesemayhaveinfluencedtheresultswereceived(regardingthepotentialimpactof genreexposure,seeFong,Mullin,&Mar,2013).Thetaskisalsonotsuitedtocapturepreviousexperiencesthatmayhave promptedourparticipantstoincludeparticularitemsintheirlists.Moreover,wecannotextrapolatewhichrepliesmayhave beenprimedbytheoreticalconceptsoftherelevantgenres(e.g.,conceptsourparticipantsacquiredinschool)andwhich oneswerearesultofownreadingexperiences.

5.3.Futureresearch

Toacquireadeeperunderstandingofhowrecipientsperceiveandconceptualizethedimensionsofaestheticappealfor differentartforms,andtoprovideamapofthedimensionsofaestheticappreciation,additionaldataforotherartistic domainswillbeindispensable.Thisincludesmultimodaldomainslikefilm,opera,performanceart,anddance,whichin manywaysbridgethegapbetweenliteratureandmusic,literatureandtheatricalperformance,andliteratureandthevisual arts.Attheotherendof thescale,itwouldbebeneficialtomakefinerdistinctionswithintheindividualdomainsand subdomains–forinstance,asubdomainlikethenovelcouldbedividedintosmallerandmorespecificsubgenres,suchas sciencefictionnovels,detectivenovels,romancenovels,andsoon.Furthermore,cross-linguisticaswellascross-cultural comparisonsforthesameartisticdomainwouldbeveryusefulinordertoestablishtheextentofvarietyweshouldaccept whentranslatingitems.Lastly,astudysuchasours,whichisbasedongeneral,openquestions,couldbecomplementedwith acorpus-basedapproachbyextractingadjectivesappraisingliteraturefromcontemporaryandhistoricalnewspapersand journals,onlineandprintreviews,blurbs,andothertypesofparatext.Anotherpossibilitywouldbeempiricalresearchthat collectsandanalyzesmorecomprehensivecommentsonthetopicofliteraturefromresearchparticipants(forastudyalong theselines,cf.Dixon&Bortolussi,2009).Inthisway,themoregeneralaestheticappealdimensionswerecordedwouldbe complementedbytermsthatreadersusetoconceptualizetheirimpressionsregardingavarietyofspecificliterarytexts.

Acknowledgments

WewishtothankMichaelBerners,JohannesBohn,NoamEshel,SabineKüster,MartinLohr,AquilesLuna-Rodriguez, AlexanderNolte,FlorianSchefenacker,AnnaWeinbrecht,andMikeWendtfortheirhelpindatacollectionandprocessing, FelixBernoullyforassistingwiththedesignofourfigures,andMariaKraxenbergerforherhelpfulcommentsonanearlier versionofthemanuscript.Apartofthisresearchwasconductedattheresearchcluster“LanguagesofEmotion”,fundedby theGermanResearchFoundation(DFG).

(13)

AppendixA.

OriginalGermaninstruction

“BitteschreibenSieWörterauf,diemanzurBeschreibungderÄsthetikvonLiteraturverwendenkann.BittebenutzenSie nurAdjektive(Eigenschaftswörter).Siehabenabjetzt2MinutenZeit.”

TableA1

Termslistedbyatleast5%oftheparticipantsinonesubsample.

English(translation) German(original)

Aesthetic ästhetisch

Amusing amüsant

Beautiful schön

Boring langweilig

Brief knapp

Charming lieblich

Comical komisch

Creative kreativ

Creepy gruselig

Deeplymoving ergreifend

Diverse abwechslungsreich

Diverting kurzweilig

Dramatic dramatisch

Emotional emotional

Entertaining unterhaltend

Entertaining unterhaltsam

Euphonious wohlklingend

Exciting aufregend

Fantastic phantastisch

Fluent fließend

Funny lustig

Harmonious harmonisch

Humorous humorvoll

Imaginative phantasievoll

Instructive lehrreich

Interesting interessant

Ironic ironisch

Long lang

Long-winded langatmig

Melancholic melancholisch

Melodic melodisch

Melodious klangvoll

Metaphorical metaphorisch

Moving bewegend

Open offen

Passionate leidenschaftlich

Poetic poetisch

Pointed pointiert

Profound tiefgründig

Realistic realistisch

Rhyming reimend

Rhythmic rhythmisch

Riveting fesselnd

Romantic romantisch

Sad traurig

Sarcastic sarkastisch

Satirical satirisch

Sentimental gefühlvoll

Short kurz

Stimulating anregend

Succinct prägnant

Surprising überraschend

Suspenseful spannend

Thrilling mitreißend

Touching berührend

Tragic tragisch

Witty witzig

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

typical substances which appear in all oils in significant amounts. Their occurrence and the analysis of characteristic components of rose hips oil in each of the investigated taxa

Since then, Canthon Goniocanthon has been considered as belonging to the tribes of Canthonini or Deltochilini because of the following character combination: dorsal surface pronotum

European Journal of Taxonomy 374: 1–23 2017 area, rectangular pseudoloculi replaced by 1–2 small, rounded ones whereas near the apices, central transapically elongated

is illustrated and discussed based on populations collected from the Vouga, Mondego and Lis river basins in central Portugal and compared with the type material of Fragilaria

Hence, the slowly sinking and presumably finer particles we measured showing characteristic -enhanced REE concentrations and strong positive Ce anomalies, closely

a School of Chemistry, Faculty of Engineering and Physical Sciences, University of Manchester, Brunswick Street, Manchester M13 9PL, England, b Department of Biology, University

TABLE 1 Average and maximum C stocks in living and dead volumes for forest registered as managed and unmanaged in Germany, based on plot data from the national forest

This work has been digitalized and published in 2013 by Verlag Zeitschrift für Naturforschung in cooperation with the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science under