• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Unattractive males can sire attractive sons in Drosophila melanogaster

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Unattractive males can sire attractive sons in Drosophila melanogaster"

Copied!
1
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Presented at the 9

th

International Congress of Neuroethology in Salamanca, August 2-7, 2010.

Unattractive males can sire attractive sons in Drosophila melanogaster

Björn Brembs, Saskia Rughöft, Kristin Leinhoß

FU Berlin, Institut für Biologie - Neurobiologie, Königin-Luise-Strasse 28/30, 14195 Berlin, Germany

bjoern@brembs.net, http://brembs.net

P 92

Courtship Wheels 5. Conclusion

Somewhat surprisingly, we observed a rather weak correlation between copulation latency and copulation frequency. Comparing our results to those of Mackay et al. (see Fig.1) it becomes clear that we did not reproduce the rank order from their experiment, i.e. their most attractive lines were not necessarily successful in our experiment. However this could be due to a number of reasons: While we used tester females from the line Canton S and observed single pairs in courtship chambers for 60 minutes, they used tester females from the line Wild Type Oregon R and observed several pairs in food vials for 2 hours.

Nevertheless, we were able to identify strains with attractive males and strains producing unattractive males for a simple genetic test. Reciprocal crosses betwen an atractive and an unattractive strain should rveal any maternal or paternal genetic contributions to attractiveness.

Our results show that the male offspring from both crosses were similar in both latency and frequency to the more attractive parent line, demonstrating that fathers from an unattractive D. melanogaster population can sire atrractive sons if they mate with females from a population that produces attractive males. This result suggests an autosomal dominant heritability of attractiveness in D. melanogaster.

To exclude spurious results due to our selection of parental strains, we will continue these experiemnts by selecting more strains for further analysis. For instance, we are currently crossing line 732 with line 360. These two lines cannot be easily classified as attractive or unattractive according to our definition, as they differ in terms of their copulation data in opposite ways (see Fig. 4).

5. Conclusion

Somewhat surprisingly, we observed a rather weak correlation between copulation latency and copulation frequency. Comparing our results to those of Mackay et al. (see Fig.1) it becomes clear that we did not reproduce the rank order from their experiment, i.e. their most attractive lines were not necessarily successful in our experiment. However this could be due to a number of reasons: While we used tester females from the line Canton S and observed single pairs in courtship chambers for 60 minutes, they used tester females from the line Wild Type Oregon R and observed several pairs in food vials for 2 hours.

Nevertheless, we were able to identify strains with attractive males and strains producing unattractive males for a simple genetic test. Reciprocal crosses betwen an atractive and an unattractive strain should rveal any maternal or paternal genetic contributions to attractiveness.

Our results show that the male offspring from both crosses were similar in both latency and frequency to the more attractive parent line, demonstrating that fathers from an unattractive D. melanogaster population can sire atrractive sons if they mate with females from a population that produces attractive males. This result suggests an autosomal dominant heritability of attractiveness in D. melanogaster.

To exclude spurious results due to our selection of parental strains, we will continue these experiemnts by selecting more strains for further analysis. For instance, we are currently crossing line 732 with line 360. These two lines cannot be easily classified as attractive or unattractive according to our definition, as they differ in terms of their copulation data in opposite ways (see Fig. 4).

2. Methods

Male virgins from 9 lines selected from the two ends of the spectrum in Fig. 1 were collected under CO2 anaesthesia and housed together for 3-4 days in food vials. They were put into courtship wheels where each male was placed into a chamber with a 3-day-old female virgin from the wild type line Canton S. As a control group, virgin males from the Canton S line were also used.

Each courtship wheel contained 12 chambers (see photos at center) and these were observed continuously for the duration of the experiment (60 minutes). During the experiment, the time elapsed between introduction of males and copulation for each pair was recorded as copulation latency, which served as our measure of male attractiveness. Simultaneously, the proportion of males achieving copulation was recorded as copulation frequency.

Using results from this experiment (experiment

#1) we chose a strain with consistently high copulation latencies and low copulation frequencies (line 313) as an unattractive parental strain. As an attractive strain, we chose line 399, which had consistently scored high on frequencies and low on latencies (see Fig. 4). These parental strains as well as the male offspring of the reciprocal crosses between the two were then tested for copulartion latency and frequency against Canton S tester females.

2. Methods

Male virgins from 9 lines selected from the two ends of the spectrum in Fig. 1 were collected under CO2 anaesthesia and housed together for 3-4 days in food vials. They were put into courtship wheels where each male was placed into a chamber with a 3-day-old female virgin from the wild type line Canton S. As a control group, virgin males from the Canton S line were also used.

Each courtship wheel contained 12 chambers (see photos at center) and these were observed continuously for the duration of the experiment (60 minutes). During the experiment, the time elapsed between introduction of males and copulation for each pair was recorded as copulation latency, which served as our measure of male attractiveness. Simultaneously, the proportion of males achieving copulation was recorded as copulation frequency.

Using results from this experiment (experiment

#1) we chose a strain with consistently high copulation latencies and low copulation frequencies (line 313) as an unattractive parental strain. As an attractive strain, we chose line 399, which had consistently scored high on frequencies and low on latencies (see Fig. 4). These parental strains as well as the male offspring of the reciprocal crosses between the two were then tested for copulartion latency and frequency against Canton S tester females.

Fig. 2: Copulation Frequency for each tested line from experiment #1.

208 399 360 313 786 820 707 CS 732 705 lines

0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 1800 2000 2200

latency [s]

CS 208 313 360

399 705 707 732

no copulation copulation

786 820

Fig. 3: Copulation Latency for each tested line from experiment #1.

3. Attractive and unattractive males

4. Offspring of reciprocal crosses are all attractive 4. Offspring of reciprocal crosses are all attractive

Fig. 3: Relationship of latency and frequency scores from experiment #1 for male offspring of the ten tested line.

Based on this data the lines for experiment #2 (313, 399) were chosen.

30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65

frequency [%]

400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 1800 2000 2200

latency [s]

208 (14)

399 (16)

707 (12) 786 (14)

360 (17) 313 (10)

732 (9)

CS (11)

705 (9) 820 (12)

35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85

frequency 400

600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 1800

latency

Aa (39/88)

Ab (43/55) Ba (58/82)

Bb (52/72)

Fig. 4: Relationship of latency and frequency scores from experiment #2 for male offspring of reciprocal crossbreeds and the parent lines (313~Aa; 399~Bb).

A,B: maternal genes a,b: paternal genes

1. Introduction

In many models of sexual selection it is assumed that attractive traits are passed on from father to son and thus also help the mother attain fitness benefits, since her sons would be more likely to father even more offspring. Taylor et al. (1) showed that this is the case in Drosophila simulans, using experiments very similar to the ones we are presenting.

However, Pischedda et al. (2) had previously concluded that "male fitness was not inherited by sons" in Drosophila melanogaster. Our goal was to further examine these conclusions.

For this purpose we decided to use some of the D. melanogaster lines whose gene expression patterns were thoroughly characterized by Mackay et al. (3). We expect to be able to use some of the data obtained in their study in the future to hopefully explain and give further insight into our results. After obtaining results about the attractiveness of 10 of those lines, we chose two lines and repeated the

1. Introduction

In many models of sexual selection it is assumed that attractive traits are passed on from father to son and thus also help the mother attain fitness benefits, since her sons would be more likely to father even more offspring. Taylor et al. (1) showed that this is the case in Drosophila simulans, using experiments very similar to the ones we are presenting.

However, Pischedda et al. (2) had previously concluded that "male fitness was not inherited by sons" in Drosophila melanogaster. Our goal was to further examine these conclusions.

For this purpose we decided to use some of the D. melanogaster lines whose gene expression patterns were thoroughly characterized by Mackay et al. (3). We expect to be able to use some of the data obtained in their study in the future to hopefully explain and give further insight into our results. After obtaining results about the attractiveness of 10 of those lines, we chose two lines and repeated the

Fig. 1: Variation for copulation latency (as one of the examined organismal phenotypes) among 40 wild-derived

inbred lines (3). lines

latency [s]

experiments with offspring obtained from both recipocal crosses. These experiments provide further information about the heritability of attractiveness in Drosophila melanogaster.

experiments with offspring obtained from both recipocal crosses. These experiments provide further information about the heritability of attractiveness in Drosophila melanogaster.

1 Taylor, M.L., Wedell, N., Hosken, D.J. (2007). The heritability of attractiveness. Curr Biol, 17(22): R959-R960.

2 Pischedda A, Chippindale AK (2006) Intralocus Sexual Conflict Diminishes the Benefits of Sexual Selection. PLoS Biol 4(11): e356. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040356

3 Mackay, T.F.C., Ayroles, J.F., Carbone, M.A., Stone, E.A., Jordan, K.W., Lyman, R.F., Magwire, M.M., Rollmann, S.M., Duncan, L.H., Lawrence, F., Anholt, R.R.H. (2009). Systems genetics of complex traits in Drosophila melanogaster.

Nature Genetics, 41(3): 299-307.

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

A memoryless source of entropy 15 bits per sourceword is connected to a binary sym- metric channel of symbol error probability p = 0.1, which can pass 1000 binary digits per

In this exercise we want to show that the model construction for FO 2 -formulae from the lecture is optimal in the following sense: in general it does not suffice to take only

Mathematische Grundlagen der Informatik RWTH

The syntax of FO is given by the usual rules together with an additional ε-rule: If ψ is a formula, and x is a variable, then εxψ is a term (read “an x such

The classical approach to generalize the technique of forcing with set-sized partial orders to forcing with class partial orders is to work with countable transitive models M of

and so by taking the preimages through f of both sides we obtain that.. Hence, applying f to both sides we

This exercise sheet aims to assess your progress and to explicitly work out more details of some of the results proposed in the previous lectures. Please, hand in your solutions

However, the widespread occurrence only colonized from South America via the Scotia Arc, two other of Cumella emergens Cobera, 2000, in the Antarctic is an example of a