• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Conditioned Discrimination of Electric Waves Differing Only in Form and Harmonic Content in the Electric Fish, Eigenmannia

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Conditioned Discrimination of Electric Waves Differing Only in Form and Harmonic Content in the Electric Fish, Eigenmannia "

Copied!
2
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Conditioned Discrimination of Electric Waves Differing Only in Form and Harmonic Content in the Electric Fish, Eigenmannia

B. Kramer and G.K.H. Zupanc

Zootogisches Institut der Universit~it, D-8400 Regensburg

The individual frequencies of the con- tinuous, wave-like electric organ dis- charges (EODs) of the South American knife fish Eigenmannia (Gymnoti- formes, Teleostei) are remarkably con- stant at stable temperatures [1, 2] with- in a species-specific range of 240--600 Hz [3]. Playback experiments using tape-recorded EODs, presenta- tions of sinusoidal electric stimuli and field observations seemed to indicate that the EOD frequency is a cue in spe- cies recognition [3, 4]. However, a later study of sympatric fish communities re- vealed, a signal's frequency is of limited use in species recognition due to exten- sive overlapping of species-specific ranges in EOD frequencies [5].

We tested whether Eigenmannia is sen- sitive to the finer detail of its EOD, especially the sexual dimorphism in waveform and harmonic content [6, 7].

The male EOD resembles short pulses superimposed on a head-negative D.C.

baseline containing higher harmonics or overtones which are strong com- pared to the fundamental (or EOD) frequency, while the female EOD al- most resembles a sine wave containing weak, higher harmonics (Fig. 1 a, b).

Four E. lineata (11.6-13.2cm), ob- tained from a tropical fish dealer, were tested during the day. The test aquar- ium (75 x 40 x 42 cm high; water con- ductivity 100 + 5 gS c m - 1 ; tempera- ture 27_+1.5 ~ was provided with a centred porous pot (length 33 cm; in- ner diameter 6.5 cm; wall 1.2 cm) the long axis of which was parallel to the smaller sides of the aquarium. An iso- lated fish remained in the porous pot most of the day (L:D, 12:12). One electric dipole made of carbon rod elec- trodes (diameter 0.5 cm; length 1.8 cm;

separation of the two vertically ori- ented carbon rods 7.8 cm) was placed parallel and close to each end of the aquarium, that is, symmetrically to the left and right of a fish hiding in its shelter. Imitations of natural EODs were digitally synthesized by a specially

designed microprocessor system [8]

and presented through one of the di- poles. A function generator provided sine and sawtooth waves. Stimulus am- plitudes (p-p) were kept constant among two signals within 5%, in a range of 240-690 gV c m - 1 at the po- sition of the fish's head in its shelter (measured with the fish removed; at least 50 dB above threshold of the con- ditioned response). Stimulus frequency was 50 Hz above the fish's EOD fre- quency in order not to evoke a jam- ruing avoidance response (JAR, [1, 2]).

Male EODs and sawtooth waves with their strong overtones contrasted with female EODs and especially sine waves with little or no harmonic content (Fig. 1, right).

The first step in a fish's training con- sisted of forming the association of

"presence of an electric signal" with a food reward (one Chironomus mos- quito larva) obtained at a feeder nearby the active dipole. Learning progress was monitored by measuring the time (s) from the onset of a sine wave signal to the fish's touching a feeder. The next step in the fish's training consisted of discriminating a rewarded signal (for example, sine waves) from an unre- warded one (for example, sawtooth waves). Mild punishment was neces- sary to establish discrimination be- cause of the fishes' tendency to ap- proach an active dipole independent of whether it presented a rewarded or an unrewarded signal. When appropriate, a fish was " p u n i s h e d " by approaching it gently with a plastic rod (without touching). A fish punished in this way returned to its shelter.

Discrimination conditioning was con- ducted in a randomized-blocks design [9]. A training block consisted of four training trials (signal no. 1, left; signal no. 1, right; signal no. 2, left; signal no. 2, right; sequence determined by random permutation) with equal numbers of presentations of each out of two signals actually in use. A subse-

quent test trial without food reward nor punishment completed one ran- domized block. The sequence of test trials was also determined by random permutation. The number of trials per day was between 30 (24 training and 6 test trials) and 40 (32 training and 8 test trials). The inter-trial interval was 5 rain. When the fish did not respond a signal was turned off after 150 s.

Stimulus amplitude was smoothly turned on or off by hand during 1/2 s.

Under these conditions discrimination was established during the first day of training in all fish.

The fish discriminated successfully be- tween all pairs of signals tested: re- warded sine waves and unrewarded sawtooth waves (P<0.01 in each of four fish tested; Fig. 2a); rewarded sine waves and unrewarded male EODs (P<0.001 in each fish; Fig. 2b); re- warded female EODs and unrewarded male EODs (P<0.01 in each fish;

Fig. 2c). A fourth experiment (not shown) consisted of reversing the roles of rewarded and unrewarded signals, as used in the second experiment, and immediately following it: now it was the dipole playing back rewarded male EODs which was approached faster than the one presenting unrewarded sine waves (P<0.025 in three out of four fish; N > 12 test trials per fish and waveform).

A simple cue enabling a fish to discrim- inate between two signals, as used in the present report, might be their dif- ferences in fundamental frequency in- tensities (compared to a sine wave. of 0 dB, fundamental frequency intensi- fies were: - 4 . 1 , - 3 . 0 , and 0 d B for the sawtooth wave, the male and fe- male EODs, respectively). A similar type of signal analysis was shown in Eigenmannia's JAR: the strength of J A R was correlated with the intensity of that stimulus harmonic which was close to the EOD fundamental fre- quency, and independent of stimulus waveform or harmonic content [7].

Therefore, in a fifth experiment the first experiment was repeated in a mod- ified way, using an attenuated sine wave to match the weaker intensity of the fundamental frequency component of the sawtooth wave. All four fish tested still discriminated the rewarded sine wave from the unrewarded saw- tooth wave of stronger peak-to-peak amplitude (P<0.01 in each fish;

Naturwissenschaften 73 (1986) 9 Springer-Verlag 1986 679

(2)

=

o =_

- 1 0 - 2 0

0 - 3 0

- 4 0

O 0 3 2 6 4 0

O 0 3 2 6 4

O 0 2 4 4 8

2 3 4 5

- 1 0 - 2 0

- 3 0 I

- 4 0 . I , . . . . .

,~, 0 1 2 3 4 5

- 1 0

- 2 0

- 3 0

0 1 2 3 4 5

~ ,

- 1 0

- 2 0 - 3 ~

O 0 2 4 4 8 0 2 3 4 5

1 t , m e [ m s ] F r e q . . . . y [ k H z ] 2

16 - 16 12 - 14 14 - 14 18 - 18

16 - 16 15 - 15 t 5 - 14 14 - 15

t

,~ 2o

0

1 5 - 1 5 1 5 - 1 6 1 4 - 1 4 1 5 - 1 5

4 0

2 0

15 - 16 13 - 14 16 - 16 18 - 18 2 0 q d

9 9 Q @ Fig. 1. Waveforms of electric signals (left) and Fourier amplitude spectra (right) as used in conditioned discrimination experiments with Eigenmannia lineata. The ordinates of the left diagrams are arbitrary linear amplitudes (V), of the right diagrams amplitudes expressed as dB attenuation relative to the strongest spectral component of each waveform, a) Electric organ discharges (EODs) of a male Eigenmannia, as generated by a microprocessor-based system for the digital synthesis of EODs [8] (from [7]), b) digitally synthesized EODs of a female Eigenmannia (from [7]), c) sine waves and d) sawtooth waves generated by a function generator, all recorded from the water

Fig. 2. Mean latencies of four isolated Eigenmannia, measured from the onset of an electric signal presented through a dipole to a fish's touching a nearby feeder, in a food-rewarded conditioned discrimination experiment. Standard errors are either shown or are too small to be drawn. Individual fish numbers are on the bottom. The number of test trials is indicated above each column. Open columns rewarded signals; shaded unrewarded signals.

The differences among all paired columns are significant at P<0.001 (except* where P < 0.01; one-tailed Mann-Whitney U-test). The rewarded and unrewarded signals, respec- tively, were in a) sine and sawtooth waves; b) sine waves and male EODs; c) female and male EODs; d) sine and sawtooth waves of matched intensities of their fundamental frequency components, hence different peak-to-peak amplitudes (unlike the other pairs of signals). Note that in each fish, its latency to approach a dipole presenting a rewarded signal was much shorter than when the dipole presented an unrewarded signal. Experiment c) shows discrimination of female from male EODs both having harmonic content. Experi- ment d) shows that the fish must have recognized categorical differences in the stimuli presented, not simply intensity differences in the fundamental frequency components

Fig. 2d). This shows t h a t the fish in- deed recognized categorical differences in the stimuli tested.

W h a t is it t h a t enables Eigenmannia to recognize categorical differences in electric signals? A c c o r d i n g to one hy- pothesis [10] the fish are sensitive for differences in the intervals between zero-crossings o f the two half-waves o f

a signal cycle. These intervals are sym- metric (equal) in a sine o r s a w t o o t h wave (Fig. 1 c, d), b u t m a r k e d l y asym- metric (unequal), for example, in the male E O D (Fig. l a). M a l e E O D s might therefore have been discrimin- a t e d from sine waves by their p a t t e r n o f zero-crossings intervals. However, t h a t type o f signal analysis can be ruled

o u t in the first a n d the fifth experiment where the fish discriminated sine waves from s a w t o o t h waves although they were identical regarding their intervals between zero-crossings. V a r i a t i o n o f intervals between zero-crossings by varying the phase difference between h a r m o n i c signal c o m p o n e n t s , t h a t is, the form o f the complex wave, did not affect the J A R [7].

Therefore, in the present study the fish p r o b a b l y noticed the differences o f the signals in h a r m o n i c content. Experi- ments in progress test the hypothesis whether Eigenmannia discriminates electric signals differing in h a r m o n i c content in a way, for example, similar to t i m b r e p e r c e p t i o n in the a u d i t o r y system (for example, [11]). One o f the sensory requirements o f timbre percep- tion, a set o f differently tuned electro- receptors in the same fish, is met ac- cording to [12] (see also discussion in

[71).

S u p p o r t e d b y the Deutsche F o r - schungsgemeinschaft ( S F B 4 / H I ) , and a student fellowship from the Friedrich- E b e r t - F o u n d a t i o n (Bonn) to G.Z.

Prof. G. Neuweiler (University o f Munich) a n d B.O. B r a t t o n k i n d l y criti- cized the manuscript.

Received April 17 and June 18, 1986 1. Watanabe, A., Takeda, K. : J. exp. Biol.

40, 57 (1963)

2. Bullock, T.H., Hamstra, R.H., Schelch, H. : J. Comp. Physiol. 77, 1 (1972) 3. Hopkins, C.D. : Behaviour 50, 270 (1974) 4. Hopkins, C.D., Heiligenberg, W.: Be-

hay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3, 113 (1978) 5. Kramer, B., Kirschbaum, F., Markl, H.,

m: Sensory Physiology of Aquatic Lower Vertebrates, p. 195 (T. Szabo, G.

Czeh, eds.). Budapest: Pergamon Press/

Akademiai Kiado 1981

6. Kramer, B.: Verh. Dtsch. Zool. Ges.

1983, 170

7. Kramer, B.: J. exp. Biol. 119, 41 (1985) 8. Kramer, B., Weymann, D. : submitted 9. Cochran, W.G., Cox, G.M. : Experimen-

tal Designs. London-Sydney: Wiley 1968

10. Gottschalk, B., in: Sensory Physiology of Aquatic Lower Vertebrates, p. 255 (T.

Szabo, G. Czeh, eds.). Budapest: Perga- mon Press/Akademiai Kiado 1981 l l . Roederer, J.G.: Introduction to the

Physics and Psychophysics of Music.

New York-Heidelberg-Berlin: Springer 1975

12. Viancour, T.A.: J. Comp. Physiol. 133, 317 (1979)

680 Naturwissenschaften 73 (1986) 9 Springer-Verlag 1986

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

Adult females gravid with eggs (N = 3) gave good responses (frequency decrease of at least 3 Hz) to —AF (stimulus frequency higher than fish frequency), but no response or only

Subharmonic stimuli of nine artificial or synthesized natural waveforms at frequencies near one-half or one-third of the EOD resting frequency elicited responses only when a

Intensity Dependence of the EOD Sound Response Figure 7 shows the relationship between intensity of sound (150 Hz) and amplitude of EOD response in Hypopomus

What is not at all clear is whether in localities where more species live together, displaying widely overlapping EOD fundamental frequency ranges (in wave species) or similar EOD

A startle response was a rapid forwards and backwards movement during which the fish never totally left its porous-pot hiding-place, b Attack responses on the dipole model

The distribution of EOD rates, displayed at the moment of physical contact, is significantly different (p &lt; 0.001, sign test) from the distribution of the

bahianus, two congeners also occurring in northeastern Brazil, by its colour pat- tern, in which the pale interbands, anterior to vertical through first ventral lateral line ramus,

We see now, the transmission is a function of θ and the maximum transmission occurs at θ = 45 ◦ , as well as losing 50 % in terms of the eld amplitude, which correspondingly