• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Gonads and strife : sex hormones vary according to sexual orientation for women and stress indices for both sexes

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Gonads and strife : sex hormones vary according to sexual orientation for women and stress indices for both sexes"

Copied!
12
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Gonads and strife: Sex hormones vary according to sexual orientation for women and stress indices for both sexes

Robert-Paul Juster

a,∗

, Daniel Almeida

b

, Christopher Cardoso

c

, Catherine Raymond

d

, Philip Jai Johnson

a

, James G. Pfaus

c

, Adrianna Mendrek

e,f

, Annie Duchesne

f

,

Jens C. Pruessner

b,g,h

, Sonia J. Lupien

e

aDepartmentofPsychiatry,ColumbiaUniversity,NewYork,NY,UnitedStates

bDepartmentofNeurologyandNeurosurgery,McGillUniversity,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

cDepartmentofPsychology,ConcordiaUniversity,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

dDépartmentdeNeuroscience,UniversitédeMontréal,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

eDépartmentdePsychiatrie,UniversitédeMontréal,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

fDepartmentofPsychology,Bishop’sUniversity,Sherbrooke,Quebec,Canada

gDepartmentofPsychiatry,McGillUniversity,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

hDepartmentofPsychology,McGillUniversity,Montreal,Quebec,Canada

Keywords:

Sexualorientation Testosterone Estradiol/progesterone

Dehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate Cortisol

Allostaticload

a b s t r a c t

Thisstudyassessedsexualorientationandpsychobiologicalstressindicesinrelationtosalivarysexhor- monesaspartofawell-validatedlaboratory-basedstressparadigm.Participantsincluded87healthy adultsthatwereonaverage25yearsoldwhoself-identifiedaslesbian/bisexualwomen(n=20),het- erosexualwomen(n=21),gay/bisexualmen(n=26),andheterosexualmen(n=20).Twosalivasamples werecollectedfifteenminutesbeforeandfifteenminutesafterexposuretoamodifiedTrierSocialStress Testtodeterminetestosterone,estradiol,andprogesteroneconcentrationsviaenzyme-immuneassay- ing.Meansexhormoneswerefurthertestedinassociationtostressindicesrelatedtocortisolsystemic output(areaunderthecurvewithrespecttoground)basedontenmeasuresthroughoutthetwo-hour visit,allostaticloadindexedusing21biomarkers,andperceivedstressassessedusingawell-validated questionnaire.Resultsrevealedthatlesbian/bisexualwomenhadhigheroveralltestosteroneandproges- teroneconcentrationsthanheterosexualwomen,whilenodifferenceswerefoundamonggay/bisexual menincomparisontoheterosexualmen.Lesbian/bisexualwomenandheterosexualmenshowedposi- tiveassociationsbetweenmeanestradiolconcentrationsandallostaticload,whilegay/bisexualmenand heterosexualwomenshowedpositiveassociationsbetweenmeantestosteroneandcortisolsystemicout- put.Insummary,sexhormonevariationsappeartovaryaccordingtosexualorientationamongwomen, butalsoasafunctionofcortisolsystemicoutput,allostaticload,andperceivedstressforbothsexes.

1. Introduction

Explorationintotheneurobiologicalcorrelatesofsexualori- entationhashadacontroversialhistory.Earlyresearchreflected homosexuality’s classification asa mental illness until 1973,at whichtimetheAmericanPsychiatricAssociationremoveditfrom itsdiagnostic manual(Friedmanand Downey, 1994).Biological

Correspondingauthorat:ColumbiaUniversity,Dept.ofPsychiatry,Divisionof BehavioralMedicineandDivisionofGender,Sexuality,andHealth,ColumbiaUni- versityMedicalCenter,622West168thSt.,PH1540G,NewYork,NY10032,United States.

E-mailaddress:rjuster2108@cumc.columbia.edu(R.-P.Juster).

explanations ofnon-heterosexual behaviorhaveoften hypothe- sized adysregulationofsex-specifichormoneprofiles,resulting in anomalies in the organizational and activational effects of thesehormonesontheneurodevelopmentofcircuitryunderlying species-specificsexualbehavior.Animalmodelsinvolvingprena- tal androgen deficits, for example,were first believed tocause malehomosexuality(Phoenixetal.,1959),whileprenatalandro- genoverabundancepresumablyresultedinfemalehomosexuality.

Ourstudyendeavourstoshowthatsuchsexhormonedifferences assumedtobeattributabletosexualorientationarealsomodulated byunexploredstressphenomena.

Advancesinbehavioralneurosciencehasledtotheintroduction ofsophisticatedgeneticmodelsinvolvingnon-functionalandrogen receptors,furtherallowingresearcherstocharacterizetherelation-

Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS) URL: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-2-1f6t7wu8qvgcl6 Erschienen in: Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 72 (2016). - S. 119-130

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.06.011

(2)

shipbetweenandrogens,themasculinizationoftherodentbrain, andsexualbehavior(Zuloagaetal.,2008).Guidedbythisliterature, perturbationsinthehumanprenatalenvironmentduringacrit- icalperiod(between10to22weeksgestation)mightexposethe humanfetustoandrogenimbalancesthathavethepotentialtoalter structureandfunctionofkeyneuroanatomicalregionsimplicated inhumansexuality(EllisandAmes,1987).Whilethisearlyexpo- sureofandrogenstothedevelopingbrainisunderstoodtoaffect post-natalsex/gender,sexualorientation,andrelatedbehaviors, theliteraturehasbeeninconclusiveandthusreflectsthemultifac- torialnatureofhumansexuality.

Several human studies during the 1970s using analytes extractedfromeitherurine,serum,orplasmarevealedthatmen belongingtoasexualminorityshowedhighertestosterone(Brodie etal.,1974;Doerretal.,1976;TourneyandHatfield,1973),lower testosterone(Brodieetal.,1974;Kolodnyetal.,1972;Kolodnyetal., 1971;Loraineetal.,1971;Loraineetal.,1970;Pillardetal.,1974;

Rohdeetal.,1977;Stahletal.,1976),nodifferencesintestosterone (Barlow et al.,1974;Birk etal., 1973;Doerret al.,1973;Jaffee etal.,1980),higherestrogen(Doerretal.,1973;Doerretal.,1976), orlowerestrogen(Evans,1972)whencomparedtoheterosexual controls. Likewise,amongsexualminoritywomen comparedto age-matchedheterosexualcontrols,studieshavereportedlower estrogen(Loraineetal.,1971;Loraineetal.,1970),highertestos- terone(Loraineetal.,1971;Loraineetal.,1970),nodifferences intestosterone(Downeyetal.,1987),andnodifferencesinestro- genor progesterone (Griffiths et al., 1974; Seyler et al., 1978).

Similarlyfor gonadotropins,studiesreporting elevatedluteiniz- inghormoneconcentrationsamongsexualminoritymen(Kolodny etal.,1972)andwomen(Loraineetal.,1971)havebeenmatched byanabundanceofresearchfailingtoshowdifferencesinluteiniz- inghormone, folliclestimulatinghormone, as wellasprolactin (FriedmanandFrantz,1977;Jaffeeetal.,1980;Kolodnyetal.,1971;

Parksetal.,1974).

Inacriticalreviewoftheliterature,Meyer-Bahlburgconcluded thatfindingswereoverallinconsistentamongsexualminoritymen (Meyer-Bahlburg,1977).Bycontrast,aboutone-thirdofparticipat- ingsexualminoritywomenmanifestedelevatedandrogenlevels while otherwise showing no endocrine abnormalities (Meyer- Bahlburg,1979).Importantly,methodologicaldifferencesbetween studiesrenderedcomparisonsandanyfinalconclusionsdifficult.

Thisbodyofhumanresearchdidnotsupporttheneurohormonal hypothesisofsexualorientation(BanksandGartrell,1995).Meyer- Bahlbergacknowledgedthatsomeearlyresearcherswererightfully cautiousintheirconclusions(Meyer-Bahlburg,1977).Inparticu- lar,theinconsistenciesinobservedHPG-axispatternsmightnot betheprimarycauseof sexualminorityorientation,but rather asecondaryconsequencerelatedtounmeasuredfactorssuchas psychosocialstress(Kolodnyetal.,1972;Meyer-Bahlburg,1979).

Stressresearchersduringthe1970sbeganshowingthatpsy- chological factors could modulate the HPG-axis. For example, a longitudinal study among military men undergoing stressful trainingrevealedthatplasmatestosteronelevelswerelowestdur- ingtheearliernovicephasecompared tothelaterseniorphase (Kreuzetal.,1972).ThisparalleledthepioneeringworkofMason who systematically studied stressful situations (e.g., parachute jumping,air-trafficcontrolling) andidentifiedkeypsychological determinants(e.g.,novelty,uncontrollability)thatactivatedstress responses(Mason,1968).Unfortunatelythisknowledgewasnot appliedtounderstandingthemixedHPG-axisliteratureonsex- ualorientation,despitespeculationthatpsychosocialstressmight beinvolvedinstudyinconsistencies.Becausesexualminoritiesare atanincreasedriskforstress-relatedpathologies duetostigma anddiscrimination(IOM,2011;Meyer,2003),itishighlyproba- blethatuniquepsychosocialcontextsinfluenceHPG-axisprofiles

andmightconfoundtheliteraturedescribingbiologicaldifferences focusingpurelyonsexualorientation,identity,&behavior.

Advancesinpsychosocialandbiologicalapproachestostudy- ing stress propelled an entire field of psychoneuroendocrine research aimedat identifying mechanisms of disease suscepti- bility.In particular,thedevelopmentof laboratory-basedstress inductionparadigms havesubstantiatedthatthesexes differin theirstressresponsepatternsofthestresshormonecortisoleas- ilycollectedviasaliva.StudiesusingthepopularTrierSocialStress TestorTSST(Kirschbaumetal.,1993)consistentlyshowthatmen mountagreatercortisolresponsethanwomenofreproductiveage (Kirschbaumetal.,1992).Inturn,womenshowfurtherattenuation whenusingoralcontraceptives(Kirschbaumetal.,1995)ordur- ingthehighestrogen(follicular)phaseoftheirmenstrualcyclesas opposedtoduringthelutealphase(Kirschbaumetal.,1999).

Beyond sex differences in stress reactivity, research apply- ing stress biomarkers are beginning to be used to understand how stigmaaffects the health and wellbeing of sexualminori- ties(Hatzenbuehleretal.,2013).Ourgrouphasrecentlyprovided novelevidencethatsexualorientationmodulatescortisolreactiv- ity.Specifically,lesbian/bisexualwomenshowhigherpost-stressor cortisolconcentrationscomparedtoheterosexualwomen,while gay/menshowoveralllowercortisolconcentrationscomparedto heterosexualmenaftercontrollingforbasalsexhormoneconcen- trations(Juster etal., 2015).In additionto this, we foundthat sexualminorityparticipantswhohaddisclosedtheirsexualorien- tationtofamilyandfriendsevidencedlowermorningcortisollevels andlesspsychiatricsymptomsthanthosewhohadnotcompletely disclosedirrespectiveofsex(Justeretal.,2013b).Thesereportssug- gestthatimportantpsychosocialandbehavioralfactorsmayresult indistinctbiologicalsignatures.

Itremainsunknownhowever,howcirculatingsexhormones inthecontextoftheTSSTparadigmvaryasafunctionofone’s sexualorientation,andhowtheseassociationsarefurthermod- ulatedby stressphenomena.In thecurrent study,weexplored whethersexualminoritiesdifferfromsame-sexheterosexualcon- trolsintermsofsalivarytestosterone,estradiol,andprogesterone concentrationsbeforeandafterexposuretotheTSST.Guidedbyour previousreportsshowingthatbiopsychosocialstressisuniquely experiencedbetweenandwithinsexualorientations(Justeretal., 2015;Justeretal.,2016a;Justeretal.,2013b),wefurtherassessed whetherchangesinsexhormoneswereassociatedwithcortisol systemicoutputsummarizedusing10measurementsthroughout theTSST,allostaticloadindexedusing21stress-relatedbiomarkers, andfinallyperceivedstress.

GiventhemixedfindingsintheHPG-axisliteratureonsexual orientationandthelackofstudieslinkedtostressindices,wedid nothypothesizedirectionalityofassociations.We did,however, hypothesizethatpsychobiologicalstressindiceswouldcorrelate withmeansexhormoneconcentrationsbeyondthoseassociations attributabletosexualorientation.

2. Methods 2.1. Participants

Eighty-sevenparticipantsages18–45(M=24.61±0.61SE)iden- tifying as lesbianor gay (8 women and 20 men), bisexual(13 womenand6men),and heterosexual(20womenand 21men) wererecruitedfromMontrealaspartofabroaderresearchpro- gram(Justeretal.,2015;Justeretal.,2016a;Justeretal.,2013b).

Toequalizegroupsduetofewerlesbiansandbisexualmen, we combinedlesbian/gaywithbisexualindividuals (20women and 26men)andcontrastedthemtoheterosexuals(20womenand21 men).

(3)

Table1

Sampledescriptivestatisticsaccordingtosexandsexualorientation.

Information Sample Lesbian/BisexualHeterosexualGay/BisexualHeterosexualp

N 87 20 20 26 21

Demographic:

Age,M(SE) 24.61(0.61) 24.10(1.34) 25.45(1.13) 23.77(0.98) 25.33(1.47) 0.685

Race/ethnicity:

White,% 70.1 75.0 55.0 73.1 76.2 0.147

Black,% 5.7 15.0 10.0 0.0 0.0 0.147

Asian,% 12.6 0.0 10.0 23.1 14.3 0.147

Hispanic,% 6.9 0.0 20.0 3.8 4.8 0.147

Arab,% 4.6 10.0 5.0 0.0 4.8 0.147

Occupation:

Workers,% 34.5 40.0 35.0 23.1 42.9 0.490

Students,% 65.5 60.0 65.0 76.9 57.1 0.490

Working/studyinghours/week,M(SE) 28.02(1.82) 28.40(3.63) 28.5(4.38) 29.96(3.24) 24.91(3.54) 0.789

SexualorientationA:

Sexualattractions,M(SE) 3.51(0.26) 4.75(0.33) 1.40(0.15) 5.96(0.30) 1.29(0.10) <0.001

Sexualbehaviors,M(SE) 3.34(0.26 4.25(0.44) 1.30(0.11) 6.00(0.29) 1.114(0.8) <0.001

Sexualfantasies,M(SE) 3.61(0.26) 5.15(0.25) 1.65(0.25) 5.81(0.32) 1.29(0.12) <0.001

Lifestylepreferences,M(SE) 3.37(0.27) 5.15(0.44) 1.115(0.08) 5.54(0.33) 1.10(0.07) <0.001

Sexualidentity,M(SE) 3.52(0.27) 5.05(0.43) 1.15(0.08) 6.04(0.26) 1.19(0.09) <0.001

Socio-economic:

Post-secondaryeducation,% 95.3 95.0 90.0 100.0 95.0 0.575

Personalannualincome,$CAD,M(SE) 16,000(0.17) 14,500(0.34) 19,000(0.53) 14,000(0.19) 16,800(0.33) 0.737 Householdannualincome,$CAD,M(SE) 32,100(0.32) 37,000(0.68) 25,000(0.54) 27,100(0.52) 39,000(0.75) 0.311 Healthandwell-being:

Medicationuse,% 18.4 25.0 15.0 15.4 14.3 0.105

Oralcontraceptiveuse,% 16.1 20.0 50.0 0.017

Minorphysicalcondition,% 34.5 50.0 40.0 23.1 28.6 0.238

Psychiatrichistory:

None,% 28.7 15.0 40.0 26.9 33.3 0.522

Pasthistory,% 8.0 15.0 5.0 11.5 33.3 0.522

Familyhistory,% 35.6 35.0 45.0 30.8 33.3 0.522

Bothpastandfamilyhistory,% 27.6 35.0 10.0 30.8 33.3 0.522

SubjectivedimensionsB:

Self-ratedhealth,M(SE) 3.72(0.09) 3.50(0.21) 3.95(0.14) 3.69(0.17) 3.75(0.20) 0.419

Self-ratedphysique,M(SE) 3.37(0.10) 3.15(0.22) 3.70(0.18) 3.27(0.14) 3.40(0.28) 0.284

Self-rateddiet,M(SE) 3.30(0.11) 3.25(0.19) 3.40(0.25) 3.34(0.19) 3.15(0.25) 0.832

Behavioral:

Tobaccosmoking:

Smokers,% 11.5 5.0 10.0 11.5 19.0 0.376

Socialsmokers,% 14.9 10.0 5.0 19.2 23.8 0.376

Non-smokers,% 73.6 85.0 85.0 69.2 57.1 0.376

Alcoholconsumption(weekly):

0orinfrequently,% 25.3 25.0 50.0 18.1 4.8 0.134

1to5,% 40.2 45.0 40.0 26.9 52.4 0.134

6to10,% 26.4 25.0 10.0 38.5 28.6 0.134

11ormore,% 8.0 5.0 0.0 11.5 14.3 0.134

Elicitdruguse:

None,% 66.7 60.0 85.0 61.5 61.9 0.334

Occasional(monthlyorannually),% 24.1 35.0 5.0 30.8 23.8 0.334

Regular(dailyorweekly),% 9.2 5.0 10.0 7.7 14.3 0.334

Interpersonal:

Single,% 72.1 60.0 80.0 80.8 65.0 0.315

Children,% 4.7 10.0 5.0 0.0 5.0 0.463

Siblings,% 86.0 95.0 85.0 80.8 85.0 0.578

Parentsalive,% 94.2 95.0 90.0 92.3 100.0 0.559

Infrequentfamilygatherings,% 36.1 30.0 30.0 57.7 20.0 0.169

Non-religious/spiritual,% 78.8 76.5 84.2 84.0 68.4 0.569

During screening, women provided information concerning theirmenstrualcycles(e.g.,dateoflastmenses,averagecycledays) andoralcontraceptiveuse.Onewomanwhodidnotprovideinfor- mationonreproductivefunctioningandwasthereforeremoved frommainanalyses.Themainexclusionarycriteriainthisstudy weremajorhealthproblems,severementalillness,oruseofsyn- theticglucocorticoids.Transgenderindividualswerenotsolicited duetohormonaltreatmentsthatdirectlyaffecttheHPG-axis;how- ever,thisrepresentsagroupthatoughttobeincludedinfuture researchgivencriticalhealthinequalities(IOM,2011)Thesample’s information on demographics, socio-economics, health, wellbe- ing,lifestylebehaviors,andinterpersonalfeaturesarereportedin Table1.

2.2. Generalprotocol

Thisstudywasapprovedbytheresearchethicsboardofthe InstitutuniversitaireensantémentaledeMontréal.Upona15-min studyexplanationandscreeninginterviewviatelephone,eligible participantswerescheduledforafirstvisitattheCentreforStud- iesonHumanStress.Allparticipantsprovidedinformedconsent uponarrivaltoourlaboratory.TestingwasconductedbetweenJuly 2010toNovember2010,constraininganyseasonalvariationthat confoundsexhormones(vanAndersetal.,2006).

Amorningvisitwasscheduledbetween8:00AMand11:00AM andlastedapproximately40min.Thisvisitinvolvedablooddraw fromacertifiednurse,aContinentalbreakfasttobreaka12hfast, completionofquestionnaires,andfinally,detailedinstructionsfor

(4)

take-homebiomarkercollectionprotocolsfirstreportedelsewhere (Justeretal.,2013b).Betweenvisitsspacedonaverageoneweek apart,participantscompletedtake-homematerialsbeforereturn- ingtothelaboratory.

Anafternoon/eveningvisitwasscheduledbetween12:00PM and7:00PMandlasted120min.Thecurrentstudyfocusesonthis afternoon/eveningvisitinwhichparticipantswereexposedtoa modifiedversionoftheTSSTwhileprovidingsalivasamples.Upon completion of study requirements, participantswere explained theirbloodresultsindetailandcompensatedwith$50CADupon debriefing.

2.3. Sexualorientation

Sexualorientationwasascertainedand cross-validatedusing three combined methods: (1) response to one of three sepa- rateadvertisementssearchingforeitherlesbian/gay,bisexual,or heterosexualparticipants;(2)askingparticipantstheiridentified sexualorientation in an open-ended mannerduring telephone screening;and(3)administrationofamodified5-itemKleinSexual OrientationScale(Kleinetal.,1990).Thisinstrumentusesa7-point Likertscalefrom1(othersexonly)to7(samesexonly)toassess sexualattractions,sexualbehaviors,sexualfantasies,lifestylepref- erence,andsexualidentity.Theentiresample’sresponseswere internallyconsistent(␣=0.982).Basedoncorrespondenceamong these three methods, sexual orientation was coded as “sexual minority”(n=46)or“heterosexual”(n=41).

2.4. Perceiveddistress

The14-itemPerceivedStressScale(Cohenetal.,1983)measures perceivedstressusinga5-pointLikertscalefrom0(never)to4(very often).Originalpsychometricpropertiesrevealedstronginternal consistency(mean␣=0.85),test–retestreliability(meanr=0.85), andevidenceofconcurrentvaliditywithdepression(meanr=0.71) andphysicalcomplaints(meanr=0.59)amongyoungstudents.In thecurrentsample,perceivedstressshowedstronginternalcon- sistency(␣=0.88).

2.5. Stressreactivityparadigm

Duringthetwo-hourafternoonvisittoourlaboratory,partici- pantswereexposedtoamodifiedversion(Andrewsetal.,2007;

Wadiwalla et al., 2010)of the Trier Social Stress Test or TSST (Kirschbaumetal.,1993).Uponaten-minuteanticipationphase, participantswereledtoaseparateroomwheretheywereaskedto deliverafive-minutemockjobinterviewfollowedbyfiveminutes of mental arithmetic in front of an unseen, ostensible behav- ioralexpertseatedbehindaone-waymirror.Theparticipantand the“behavioralexpert”communicatedviaanintercommunication deviceandtheparticipant’sperformancewasrecordedbyavideo camera.Aseminalmeta-analysis(DickersonandKemeny,2004) posits that laboratory-based stressors eliciting social-evaluative threatincludeevaluativeaudiences,negativesocialcomparison(s), and/orrecordedperformancethatmaximizeHPA-axisreactivity.

Previous studiesby ourgroup demonstratethat placingthe evaluativeaudiencebehindaone-waymirror(‘panel-out’)further maximizes between-sexdifferences in cortisolstress reactivity.

Specifically,menexposedtothistype oftheTSSTshownosig- nificant differences in HPA-axis reactivity when compared to thestandard performance in front of theaudience (‘panel-in’);

(Andrewsetal.,2007).Incontrast,heterosexualwomenexposedto the‘panel-out’conditionshowdecreasedcortisolstressreactivity incomparisontoheterosexualwomeninthe‘panel-in’condition (Wadiwallaetal.,2010).Ourrecentstudyemployingheterosex- ualwomeninthe‘panel-out’conditionalsoreports comparable

decreased cortisolreactivity(Marin etal., 2012); however,it is unknownhowthisisrelatedtosexhormonevariations.

2.6. Visitorder

Participantsvisitedourlaboratorytwice:(1)morningvisitfor ablooddrawtoassessallostaticloadand(2)afternoonvisitfor theTSST.Visitorderwascounterbalancedrandomlytomanipulate experiencednoveltyofthetestingenvironment.Inthefirstgroup (morning/afternoon;n=49),participantsreceivedablooddrawin themorningduringtheirfirstvisitandwereexposedtotheTSSTin theafternoonduringtheirsecondvisitaboutaweeklater;thiswas reversedforthesecondgroup(afternoon/morning;n=37).Because thesecondgroupwasarrivingforthefirsttimetoourlaboratory whenexposedtotheTSST,weexpectedthattheywouldbemore distressedthanthefirstgroupwhohadalreadyfamiliarizedthem- selveswiththesetting.Thisacclimationisbelievedtohelpdiminish participants’distressexperiencedinnoveltestingenvironments (Sindietal.,2013)

Whilethisdidnotsignificantlymodulatecortisolconcentrations inapreviousreport(Justeretal.,2015),preliminaryanalyseswere conductedinthecurrentstudytoassesswhetherthismanipula- tionmodulatedsexhormonedynamics.TheHPG-axisisbelievedto beinvolvedinanticipatorystatesandexpectationofpreoperative stress(Gerraetal.,2000).Additionallynoteworthyintermsofday- timevariability,ithasbeensuggestedthattestosteronecollection shouldbeconductedintheafternoonandeveningtoprovidethe bestpossiblerepresentationofanindividual’sgeneralresponsive- nesstosocialinteractionsandnotthosebasedonnaturaldiurnal variation(Grayetal.,2004).

2.7. Salivacollectionandendocrineassays

Twosalivasamples(approximately2mLpersample)werecol- lected−15minpre-TSSTand+15minpost-TSSTtoassesssalivary testosterone,estradiol,andprogesteroneateachtime-point.Col- lectionwasachievedusingthepassivedroolmethodguidedwith sterilizedstraws.Sampleswerefrozenimmediatelyuponcollec- tionat−20Cand assayswereperformedwithinthreemonths of testingto prevent degradationknown toinfluenceHPG-axis biomarkers (Granger et al., 2004). Because collection occurred between12:00PMand7:00PM(M=2:34PM,SE=0.11),weassessed forcircadianvariationinsexhormonevaluesfortheentiresam- ple:nosignificantcorrelationswitharrivaltimewerefound.Thirty minutespriortosalivasampling,participantswereinstructedto avoidmajormeals,cigarettes,caffeinated/sugarybeverages,and dairyproducts.Theywerefurthermoreaskedtorefrainfromoral hygieneandstrenuousphysicalactivitytwohoursbeforesampling.

Tensaliva samplesdestined for cortisoldetermination were collected in ten-minute intervals at the following time-points:

−40min, −30min, −20min, −10min (anticipation phase), and immediatelybeforetheTSST,aswellas+10min,+20min,+30min, +40min,and+50minthereafter.Attheendofthetestingsessions, thesesalivasampleswerefrozenat−20 untilassaying.Apre- viousreportusingthissampleshowedthatlesbian/bisexualhad highercortisolconcentrationsthanheterosexualwomen+40min post-TSST,while gay/bisexualmen showedlower overallcorti- solconcentrationsthanheterosexualmenthroughoutthesession (Justeretal.,2015).Inthecurrentanalysis,wesummarizeall10 cortisoltime-pointstoassessassociationswithmeansexhormone concentrations.

All saliva samples were analyzed at the Centre for Studies onHumanStress(www.humanstress.ca).Priortoassayingeach biomarker,frozensampleswerebroughttoroomtemperatureto becentrifugedat1500xg(3000rpm)for15min.Fortestosterone determination,weusedanexpandedrangeenzymeimmuneassay

(5)

kit(Salimetrics®,StateCollege,PA,CatalogueNo.1-2402)where the lower limit of sensitivity is 1pg/ml. For estradiol, or more specifically17␤-estradiol,determination, weused ahighsensi- tivityenzymeimmuneassaykit(Salimetrics®,StateCollege,PA, CatalogueNo.1-3702)wheretherangeofdetectionis1–32pg/ml.

Forprogesteronedetermination,weusedahighsensitivityenzyme immuneassaykit(Salimetrics®,StateCollege,PA,CatalogueNo.1- 1502)wherethelowerlimitofsensitivityis5pg/ml.Inter-assay andintra-assay coefficientsofvariancewererespectivelybelow 12.80% and6.34%for testosterone,3.7%and6.78% forestradiol, and8.08%and6.78%forprogesterone.Forcortisoldetermination, weusedahighsensitivityenzymeimmuneassaykit(Salimetrics® StateCollege,PA,CatalogueNo.1-3102)wheretherangeofdetec- tionisbetween0.012–3ug/dL.Assayswereruninduplicatesand averageswereusedinstatisticalanalyses.

2.8. Allostaticloadbiomarkers

Allostaticloadisdefinedasthemulti-systemic‘wearandtear’

thatchronicstressandunhealthybehaviorsexactonthebodyand brain(McEwenandStellar,1993).Twenty-onebiomarkerswere usedtocalculateacount-basedallostaticloadindexconstructed accordingtothesample’sdistributionforhigh-riskpercentilesas previouslydone(Seemanetal.,1997).Systemicbiomarkersrepre- sentneuroendocrine(salivarycortisolwastransformedintoboth AMcortisol and PMcortisolslopes tocapturedynamic diurnal HPA-axisfluctuations;serumdehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate;

12h overnighturinary adrenalin, noradrenalin, and dopamine), immune/inflammatory(plasmainterleukin-6andtumour-necrosis factor-alpha;serumc-reactiveprotein;andserumfibrinogenpre- servedwithsodiumcitrate),metabolic(serumalbumin,creatinine, insulin, glycosylated haemoglobin (%), total cholesterol, high- densitylipoprotein,andtriglycerides;waist-to-hipratiomeasured withgraduatedtapeandcalculatedbydividingrespectiveinches;

bodymassindexcalculatedasmass(kg)dividedbyheight(m2);

and cardiovascular (mean of three seated systolic and diastolic bloodpressurerecordingsmeasuredwithanelectronicsphygmo- manometer;A&DMedical©:ModelUA-631V)functioning.Table2 listsallbiomarkersusedwiththeirrespectivecut-offsandgroup characterizations.

Previous allostaticload findings in this sample showedthat gay/bisexualmenhadlowerallostaticloadlevelsthanheterosex- ualmenandthatthiswasdrivenbylowertriglycerides,bodymass indices,andtrendingtumour-necrosisfactor-alphaconcentrations (Justeretal.,2013a).Amongsexualminorities,thosethatenacted avoidancecopingstrategiesduringsexualidentityformationand disclosureevidencedelevatedallostaticloadandpsychosocialdis- tress(Justeretal.,2016a).Inthecurrentanalysis,ourrationalefor usingallostaticloadwastoassesspotentialconvergenceofassocia- tionswithsalivarysexhormonesandtoassessdifferencesbetween women for individualbiomarkers which wedidnot previously assesssincewomendidnotshowallostaticloaddifferences.

2.9. Statisticalanalysis

AllstatisticalanalyseswererunusingtheStatisticalPackage for the Social Science Version 22 for Macintosh. Group differ- encesindescriptiveinformationarereportedinTable1.Groups only differedaccordingtosexuality asexpected(all ps<0.001), while heterosexualwomen usedoral contraceptivesmore than lesbian/bisexualwomen(p=0.017).

Due tothe vast inter-individual differences in sex hormone concentrations and intra-individual variations as a function of contextual stimuli and reproductive considerations, we care-

fullyaccounted for potentialconfounders in ourmain analyses Table2 Allostaticloadbiomarkerinformation. ABiomarker(Unit)ReferenceCut-OffFreq.SampleM(SE)Lesbian/BisexualM(SE)HeterosexualM(SE)Gay/BisexualM(SE)HeterosexualM(SE) CortisolAM(Slope)N/A0.03292or0.19069200.06161(0.01599)0.04172(0.02739)0.07145(0.02727)0.07445(0.03772)0.05396(0.03018) CortisolPM(Slope).N/A0.06902or0.00294180.03202(0.00431)0.03481(0.00959)0.02995(0.00635)0.02843(0.00964)0.03613(0.00808) Dehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate(mol/L)1.2–10.4<3.5218.49(0.40)7.34(0.64)5.58(0.37)10.19(0.73)10.20(0.83) Adrenalin(nmol/L)0–110>82.52337.18(6.17)55.89(26.71)29.40(3.84)33.77(3.95)31.60(2.97) Noradrenalin(nmol/L)70–475>373.7522168.14(10.57)181.37(35.19)171.10(17.69)161.77(14.36)160.90(17.41) Dopamine(nmol/L)420–2600>2055.0211681.53(76.58)1647.05(114.94)1685.30(150.15)1826.62(162.18)1521.90(167 Tumour-necrosisfactor-(pg/mL)>8>6.0233.45(0.91)2.20(0.34)2.14(0.49)2.02(0.14)7.68(3.65) Interleukin-6(pg/mL)>2.5>1.875220.93(0.16)0.63(0.10)1.09(0.45)0.86(0.29)1.16(0.38) C-reactiveprotein(mg/L)0–5>3.75202.74(0.93)3.13(1.85)2.16(1.07)1.41(0.83)4.48(3.15) Fibrinogen(g/L)2.28–5.4>4.62202.98(0.08)3.23(1.85)3.14(0.08)2.74(0.14)2.99(0.17) Insulin(pmol/L)19–154>120.252148.02(2.81)50.98(6.42)44.80(5.08)44.61(4.61)52.64(6.68) Glycosylatedhaemoglobin(%)0.042–0.062>0.057260.515(0.00022)0.052(0.00043)0.052(0.00037)0.051(0.00044)0.051(0.00042) Creatinine(mol/L)54–110>96.02368.28(1.19)62.00(2.25)62.10(2.84)72.81(1.54)74.24(1.69) Albumin(g/L)35–50<38.752644.22(0.60)41.84(0.76)42.20(0.78)46.77(1.56)45.14(0.74) Triglycerides(mmol/L)0.5–1.7>1.4210.85(0.06)0.88(0.18)0.68(0.06)0.76(0.07)1.08(0.14) Totalcholesterol(mmol/L)2–5.2>4.4224.27(0.09)4.22(0.19)4.63(0.18)4.05(0.14)4.24(0.19) Highdensitylipoproteincholesterol(mmol/L)1.05–1.55<1.175231.44(0.05)1.63(0.10)1.68(0.13)1.23(0.05)1.22(0.07) Systolicbloodpressure(mmHg)90–140>127.522113.05(1.22)111.42(1.95)107.65(2.20)116.67(2.42)115.37(2.63) Diastolicbloodpressure(mmHg)60–90>82.52269.93(0.87)71.58(1.69)68.53(1.67)69.53(1.48)70.22(2.21) Waist-hipratio(ratio)0.8–1>0.95220.83(0.12)0.824(0.015)0.081(0.010)0.843(0.009)0.822(0.043) 2Bodymassindex(kg/m)18.5–25>23.3752222.95(0.46)23.53(1.08)21.43(1.09)22.41(0.51)24.52(1.00) ANote:Clinicalreferencerangesaccompanyingbloodresultswereusedtodeterminehigh-riskcut-offsandarereportedelsewhere(Justeretal.,2016a,b;Justeretal.,2013a,b).

(6)

of repeated measures. Specifically for each sex hormone in sequence,potentialconfoundingeffectswerefirstassessedusing repeated-measures ANOVAs as a function of (1) visit order (morning/afternoon: n=49; afternoon/morning group: n=37), (2) menstrual cycle divided equally according to follicular les- bian/bisexual women (n=11) and heterosexual women (n=11) aswellasluteallesbian/bisexualwomen(n=9)andheterosexual women(n=9),andfinally(3)oralcontraceptiveuse(users:n=14;

non-users:n=26) thatwasmore commonamong heterosexual women (users: n=10, non-users: n=10) than lesbian/bisexual women (users: n=4;non-users:n=16). Using information that contrastedtheTSSTvisitdateandthedateoflastmenstruation forwomen(M=18.25,SE=2.01),preliminaryone-wayANOVAsof studyoutcomesasafunctionofmenstrualcyclestatusrevealed nosignificantdifferencesbetweenwomeninthefollicular(days 1–14)andluteal(days15–28)phases.Notethatmenstrualstatus wasunknownforoneparticipant,sofinalsampleN=86.

Mixed-design repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was run with sexual orientation entered as the between-subjects factor and sex hormones (two measures of testosterone, estradiol, and progesterone each 15min before and 15min after TSSTexposure) entered asthe within-subject factorsinsex-specificanalyseswhilecontrollingforageandvisit order as well as menstrual cycle status and oral contraceptive useamongwomen.Post-hocanalysesemployedone-wayANOVA or paired-samplet-test as required. In all such analyses, effect sizesare reportedthroughout andcanbeinterpretedaccording tothefollowingconventions:2P∼=0.01representsasmalleffect, 2P∼=0.06amediumeffect,and2P∼=0.14constitutealargeeffect (Fritzetal.,2012).

Next, we assessed associations among mean sex hormones in relationtocortisolsystemic output,allostatic load,and per- ceivedstressusing bivariatecorrelationssplit accordingtosex.

Time-dependentstressreactivecortisolconcentrationsweretrans- formedintosummaryscoresbasedontheareaunderthecurve formulaebasedonthetrapezoidformula(Pruessneretal.,2003).

Theareaunderthecurvewithrespecttoground(AUCg)wascalcu- latedtorepresentsystemicoutputthroughouttheTSSTvisit.

Insupplementalanalyses,wefollowedwithanexplorationof 21individualbiomarkerdifferencesusingANCOVAscontrollingfor ageandbivariatecorrelationswithotherstudyvariables.

3. Results 3.1. Testosterone

Inpreliminaryanalysesofvisitorder,theafternoon/morning groupshowedhighersalivarytestosteroneconcentrationsamong both women (F(1,38)=7.003, p=0.012, 2P=0.156) and men (F(1,44)=7.645,p=0.008,2P=0.148)incomparisontothemorn- ing/afternoon group.A time effect was alsofoundamong men (F(1,44)=12.241,p<0.001,2P=0.218),revealingthattestosterone concentrationssignificantlydecreasedfollowingtheTSST.Among women,menstrualstatuswasnotrelatedtodifferencesinconcen- trationsoftestosterone(p=0.26).Bycontrast,oralcontraceptive usewasrelatedtolowertestosteroneconcentrationsincomparison tonon-use(F(1.38)=6.267,p=0.017,2P=0.142).

For women (Fig. 1A), lesbian/bisexual women showed higher testosterone concentrations than heterosexual women (F(1,34)=8.648, p=0.006, 2P=0.203). Covariation effects were foundfor visitorder (F(1,34)=4.182,p=0.049,2P=0.110) while trending for oral contraceptive use (F(1,34)=3.591, p=0.067, 2P=0.096) and time X menstrual cycle phase (F(1,34)=3.044, p=0.090,2P=0.082).

For men (Fig. 1B), no significant within-subjects effects or interactionsweredetected(ps>0.364).Between-subjectsresults revealednogroupdifferences(ps>0.15)otherthanthecovarying effectofvisitorder(F(1,42)=4.892,p=0.032,2P=0.104).

3.2. Estradiol

Preliminary analyses demonstrated that visit order was not significantly related to salivaryestradiol concentrations among women(p=0.120)andmen(p=0.121)despiteatrendtowardsa timebygroupinteractionamongwomen(F(1,38)=2.991,p=0.092, 2P=0.073)andatrendtowardsatimeeffectinmen(F(1,44)=3.411, p=0.071, 2P=0.072) marked in both cases by non-significant decreasesinresponsetotheTSST.Estradiolconcentrationsdidnot differaccordingtomenstrualcycle(p=0.508)oraccordingtooral contraceptiveuse(p=0.233)amongwomen.

Forwomen(Fig.1C),nobetween-subjectseffects(ps>0.41)or within-subjectseffectsorinteractions(ps>0.18)weredetectedfor estradiolconcentrationswiththeexceptionofavisitordercovari- ationeffect(F(1,34)=4.881,p=0.034,2P=0.126).

Formen(Fig.1D), nobetween-subjectseffects(ps>0.183)or within-subjectseffectsorinteractions(ps>0.319)weredetected forestradiolconcentrations.

3.3. Progesterone

Inpreliminaryanalyses,theafternoon/morninggroupshowed higher salivary progesterone concentrations than the morn- ing/afternoon group among women (F(1,38)=4.083, p=0.050, 2P=0.097)butnotmen(p=0.272).Forwomen,nogroupdiffer- encesinprogesteroneconcentrationsweredetectedasafunction ofmenstrualstatus(p=0.776)ororalcontraceptivesuse(p=0.282).

For women (Fig. 1E), lesbian/bisexual women displayed higher progesterone concentrations than heterosexual women (F(1,34)=4.084,p=0.05,2P=0.107).BeyondatimeXmenstrualsta- tuscovariationinteraction(F(1,34)=3.772,p=0.06,2P=0.10).No otherbetween-subjectseffects(ps>0.17)orwithin-subjectseffects orinteractions(ps>0.13)weredetected.

For men (Fig. 1F), nosignificant findings were detected for between-subjecteffects (ps>0.224) or within-subject effects or interactions(ps>0.388).

3.4. Sex-specificassociationsamongmeansexhormonesand stressindices

Table3reportsalldescriptivestatisticsandcorrelativestatis- ticsfor meansexhormones,cortisol systemicoutput,allostatic load,andperceivedstressstratifiedaccordingtosex.First,testos- teronewaspositivelyassociatedwithcortisolsystemicoutputfor bothsexesandwithperceivedstressformen.Second,estradiolwas positivelyassociatedwithallostaticloadandperceivedstressonly amongmen. Third,progesteronewaspositivelyassociatedwith cortisolsystemicoutputforbothsexes.

Re-analysessplitfurther bysexand sexualorientationwere nextperformed.Forlesbian/bisexualwomen,allostaticloadwas positivelyassociatedwithestadiol(r=0.458,p=0.049).Forhetero- sexualwomen,cortisolsystemicoutputwaspositivelyassociated withtestosterone(r=0.629,p=0.001)andprogesterone(r=0.483, p=0.012). For gay/bisexual men, cortisol systemic output was positivelyassociated with testosterone (r=0.501,p=0.024). For heterosexualmen,allostaticloadwaspositivelyassociatedwith estradiol(r=0.508,p=0.022)andperceivedstresswaspositively associated with testosterone (r=0.676, p=0.001) and estradiol (r=0.457,p=0.043).

(7)

-15 +15 40

60 80 100 120 140

Lesbian/Bisexual Women Heterosexual Women

**

Time

Testosterone (pg/ml)

-15 +15

2 3 4 5 6

Lesbian/Bisexual Women Heterosexual Women

Time

Estradiol (pg/ml)

-15 +15

0 50 100 150 200

Lesbian/Bisexual Women Heterosexual Women

*

Time

Progesterone (pg/ml)

-15 +15

40 60 80 100 120 140

Gay/Bisexual Men Heterosexual Men

Time

Testosterone (pg/ml)

-15 +15

2 3 4 5 6

Gay/Bisexual Men Heterosexual Men

Time

Estradiol (pg/ml)

-15 +15

0 50 100 150 200

Gay/Bisexual Men Heterosexual Men

Time

Progesterone (pg/ml)

A B

C D

E F

Fig.1.Sex-specificestimatedmean(±SE)salivarytestosterone(AandB),estradiol(CandD),andprogesterone(EandF)—15minbeforeand+15minafterexposuretothe TrierSocialStressTest.Valuesareadjustedforageandvisitorderforbothsexesaswellasmenstrualstatusandoralcontraceptionforwomen.Note:**=p<0.01;*=p<0.05.

(8)

Table3 Descriptivestatisticsandcorrelationcoefficientsstratifiedaccordingtosex. WomenMen 1.2.3.4.5.6.7.1.2.3.4.5.6.7. 1.SexualOrientationA 2.Testosterone(M).586***0.66 3.Estradiol(M)0.060.314*0.1000.213 4.Progesterone(M).434**.425**.429**0.063.349*.664*** 5.CortisolSystemicOutput(AUCg)0.245.428**0.198.382*0.018.496***0.004.382** 6.AllostaticLoad0.1620.0150.0340.1550.1140.2780.140.333*0.1870.044 7.PerceivedStress0.0540.0640.0600.0700.0480.1790.033.455**.404**0.2390.0840.183 M84.236.58144.2311.252.3325.0168.555.99104.4912.302.7624.85 SD30.712.35111.434.881.496.7140.611.958.87.01.881.88 A=sexualorientationwascodedas0=heterosexualand1=homosexual/bisexual;AUCg=areaunderthecurvewithrespecttoground;M=mean;SD=standarddeviation. <0.10. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

3.5. Supplementalresultsforindividualsbiomarkersamong women

Individualbiomarkerscomprisingtheallostaticloadindexwere assessed toexplore convergence of salivarysex hormone find- ingsreportedabovewithotherbiomarkersextractedfromblood, urine,etc.AnalysisemployedseparateANCOVAscontrollingfor ageamong women only. Results revealedthat lesbian/bisexual women had higher levels of dehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate (F(1,36)=5.31, p=0.027,2P=0.129; Fig.2A)and lowerlevels of LDL-cholesterol(F(1,36)=4.155,p=0.049,2P=0.103;Fig.2B)than heterosexualwomen.

4. Discussion

Thisstudy exploredwhetherunbound salivarytestosterone, estradiol,andprogesteroneconcentrationsvaryasafunctionof sexualorientationaswellasbyacomprehensivearrayofstress indices. We foundthat lesbian/bisexualwomen showed higher overalltestosteroneandprogesteroneconcentrationsasagroup in comparison to heterosexual women, while menshowed no differencesinHPG-axisfunctioning.Insupplementalanalyses,les- bian/bisexualwomen alsoshowedhigherconcentrations ofthe androgenprecursorDHEA-SandlowerLDL-cholesterolthanhet- erosexualwomen.Besidesthesesexualorientationeffectsamong women vis-à-vis basal biomarkers, we also found that mean sexhormoneswereassociatedtostressindices;namely,cortisol systemicoutput,allostaticloadcomprising21biomarkers,andper- ceivedstress.Furthermore,ourfindingssuggestthatrecalibrations inHPG-axisphysiologymaybeexplainedbyintertwinedpsychoso- cialprocesses(e.g.,perceivedstress),concomitantbiobehavioral activities(e.g.,cortisoldynamics), and cumulative physiological dysregulations(e.g.,allostaticload)ratherthanstrictlyaccording tosexualorientation.

Independentofsexandsexualorientation,meansexhormones variedaccordingtostressindices.First,cortisolsystemicoutput representing10repeatedmeasurementsthroughouttheTSSTvisit was positively associated with mean testosterone and proges- teronelevelsforwomenandmen.Second,wefindnovelevidence that allostaticloadindexed with21 biomarkerswas correlated withestradiolonlyamongmen.Third,perceivedstresswaspos- itivelyassociatedwithtestosteroneandestradiolonlyamongmen.

Whenbrokendownaccordingtosexandsexualorientationgroups, lesbian/bisexualwomen and heterosexual men showedsimilar positive associations betweentestosterone and cortisol (AUCg), whilegay/bisexualmenandheterosexualwomenshowedposi- tiveassociationsbetweenestradiolandAL.Thisintriguing“gender reversal” in directionalassociations is consistent withour cor- tisolreactivityfindingsinthis samplewherebylesbian/bisexual womenhyper-reactedlikeheterosexualmenwhilegay/bisexual menhypo-reactedakintoheterosexualwomen(Justeretal.,2015).

Our findings among women contribute to a mixed liter- ature. Specifically, higher basal testosterone concentrations in lesbian/bisexualwomenis inaccordwithsomeexisting reports (Loraineetal.,1971; Loraineet al.,1970), butalsoindisaccord withstudiesshowingnodifferences(Downeyetal.,1987;Griffiths etal.,1974).Lesbian/bisexualwomen alsodemonstratedhigher basalprogesteroneconcentrationsincomparisontoheterosexual women.Whilethisresultisnovelforwomenbelongingtoasex- ualminoritygroup,astudyof92heterosexualwomen(Fleischman etal.,2015)foundthathomoeroticmotivationwassignificantly andpositivelyassociated withsalivaryprogesteronelevels.It is important tonote,however, that thevariation in progesterone concentrations varied considerably. In addition, we also found forthefirsttimethat lesbian/bisexualwomen showedelevated

(9)

0 2 4 6 8 10

Sexual Orientati on

*

Lesbian/Bisexual Women Heterosexual Women

0 1 2 3

Sexual Orientati on

LDL- Choles ter ol (mmol/l )

*

Lesbian/Bisexual Women Heterosexual Women

A B

Fig.2. Women-specificestimatedmean(±SE)(A)dehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate(DHEA-S)and(B)low-density-lipoprotein(LDL)-cholesterolbetweenlesbian/bisexual womenandheterosexualwomen.

Note:*=p<0.05.

concentrationsof serumDHEA-Sandreduced concentrationsof LDL-cholesterolaspartofblooddrawonaseparatedayfromthe TSSTsession. Thisprovidessomeconvergenceof evidencewith salivarysteroidhormone datathatstrengthensourconclusions.

Inaddition,DHEA-Swaspositivelycorrelatedwithtestosterone, whichisanexpecteddirectionofassociationgiventhebiochemical synthesisoftestosteronefromDHEA-Sthroughandrostenedione.

Alongstandingproposalinneuroendocrinologyhasbeenthat elevated androgens among sexual minority women might be explainedby environmentalstress(Meyer-Bahlburg,1979), and this may also be true of DHEA-S. Similar to cortisol, serum concentrationsofDHEA-Sareelevatedinresponsetoacutepsy- chosocialstressinducedbytheTSST(Lennartssonetal.,2012b).

Giventhatwealsofoundthathigherpost-stressorcortisolreac- tivityin lesbian/bisexualwomen incomparisontoheterosexual women (Juster et al., 2015), as well as the antagonizing rela- tionship between DHEA-S and cortisol, it is possible that the uniquepsychosocialcontextsdrivingthisendocrinephenomenon maysubsequentlyraisebasalDHEA-Sinacompensatoryfashion.

Finally,thesynthesisofsteroidhormonesdependsinpartonLDL- cholesterol, which explainswhylevels werelow among sexual minoritywomen.

Our findings among lesbian/bisexual women converge with literature suggesting a higher exposure to elevated prenatal androgensotherwisenotpresentamongmen.Forexample,pre- natal androgen exposure acts to reduce otoacoustic emissions (OAE;echo-likewaveformsemittedbythecochlea)(McFadden, 2002)andtodecreasethe2D:4Dfingerratio(Manning,2002).A meta-analysisshowedthat heterosexualwomen havea greater (morefeminine)2D:4Dratiothanlesbianwomen,butnodiffer- ences emerge for men(Grimbos et al., 2010).Lesbian/bisexual womenalsoshowdecreasedOAEs(McFaddenandPasanen,1998).

Furthermore, prenatal exposure to exogenous estrogens (e.g., diethylstilbestrol)inwomenisassociatedwithnon-heterosexual proclivities(Ehrhardtetal.,1985;Meyer-Bahlburgetal.,1995),but inconsistentlyamongmen(Hines,2011).Inthecontextofprena- talstress,oneproposalwhythesesexhormoneassociationsare notpresentinmenmaybeduetotesticularandrogenproduction thatcompensatesforstressalterationsinandrogensecretionthat isabsentamongwomen(Hines,2011).Whileprenatalconditions alone likely cannotdetermine sexual orientation(Ehrhardtand Meyer-Bahlburg,1981),thehigherandrogensamonglesbiansin ourstudy(Fig.1A)conformwiththisnotionthatdifferencesinpre-

natalandrogenexposuremightaccountforsomesexualminorities amongwomen,butnotamongmen(Fig.1B).

Regardlessofsexualorientation,menshowedunprecedented decreasesintestosteronefollowingtheTSST.Studiescontrasting timechangesintestosteronefollowingtheTSSTreportnodiffer- encesat+10minor+25min(SchoofsandWolf,2011),butthen increasedtestosteronewasdetectedat+30min(Gerraetal.,2000), at+40min(Lennartssonetal.,2012a),andfinallythroughoutper- sonalizedpeaklevelsrangingfrom+25min,+47min,and+77min (Bedgoodetal.,2014).Ourfindingofdecreasedtestosteronefrom

−15minpre-TSSTto+15minpost-TSSTissurprisingly fast.One possibilityisrapidautonomicregulationand/oranticipatorymod- ulation(e.g.,visitorder)ofstressresponsesignallingthatdampens testosterone concentrations for men. Despite noclearevidence thatluteinizinghormonediffersamongLGBindividuals,luteinizing releasinghormonesecretionisregulatedbyhypothalamicneurons thataremodulatedbyfast-actingcatecholamines(Goorenetal., 1990).Alternatively,themainmethodologicaldifferencebetween previousHPG-axisTSSTstudiesandourstudyisthatweusedthe

‘panelout’TSSTvariation wheretheevaluativejudge ishidden behindaone-waymirror.Althoughpurelyspeculative,thelackof visualsocial-evaluativethreatmightdecreasetestosteroneamong menandcouldbeexploredfurtherinthestressreactivityliterature.

Given that sex hormones are characterized by situational- dependent release, and are synergized by other biobehavioral functions(NewmanandJosephs,2009;Turanetal.,2014),consti- tutionalfactorsandsocialbehaviorsmightmodulatesexhormone physiology.Forinstance,testosteroneandcortisolinteracttopre- dictaggressiveanddominantbehaviors(CarréandMehta,2011).

Highlevelsofsexualminoritystresscoupledwithinadequatecop- ingresourcesamongstlesbianandbisexualwomen(IOM,2011) mayrecalibrateHPGaxisequilibriatofavouranendocrinemilieu underlyingalternativecopingstrategies.Futurestudiesmustassess thesepsychosocialconstructsinbetterdetailthanthecurrentstudy allowed.Bycontrast,theabsoluteabsenceofdifferencesinsexhor- monesamongmenisremarkablyconsistentwithexistingliterature (Meyer-Bahlburg,1977).

While males who identify with a sexual minority iden- tity/orientationfacesimilarpressuresandstigma,thereisempirical andtheoreticaljustificationstoconsidertheircopingmechanisms separatefromsexualminoritywomen(Lewisetal.,2012).Con- sideringthisframework,alesbian/bisexualspecificup-regulation oftestosteroneasaresultofauniquecombinationofpsychoso-

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

In the present retrospective study, it will be therefore investigated whether patients with nasal polyposis and ASA intolerance have lowered plasma levels of estradiol, pro-

If agents (a) present their efforts as contri- butions to jointly achieved development outcomes, and if agents (b) are willing to be creative, and to enrich visibil- ity

Over a number of years, research that has concentrated upon decision-making in relation to engaging in &#34;risky&#34; sexual activity has shown that, lacking adequate

Thus, we measured odor discrimination ability, empathy-related measures, and facial mimic activity (also associated with affective empathy- related measures) in free-cycling women

Vanilla- and strawberry ice cream with marinated fruit salad, natural yoghurt, cream, raspberry sauce 7,20 €.

Gender Non-Conforming is an adjective and umbrella term to describe individuals whose gender expression, gender identity, or gender role differs from gender norms associated

Adolescents with significant levels of psychiatric risk (i.e., reporting suicidal ideation or an attempt), may require mental health treatment, hence the significant association

Consequently, the range and median prevalence estimates across states and large urban school districts for the prevalence of having ever used hallucinogenic drugs by sexual