• Keine Ergebnisse gefunden

Double Dissociation of Protein Kinase C and Adenylyl Cyclase Manipulations on Operant and Classical Learning in Drosophila

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Aktie "Double Dissociation of Protein Kinase C and Adenylyl Cyclase Manipulations on Operant and Classical Learning in Drosophila"

Copied!
1
0
0

Wird geladen.... (Jetzt Volltext ansehen)

Volltext

(1)

Presented at the Neurofly 2008 conference in Würzburg, Germany, September 9, 2008.

Double Dissociation of Protein Kinase C and Adenylyl

Cyclase Manipulations on Operant and Classical Learning in Drosophila

Björn Brembs

FU Berlin, Institut für Biologie - Neurobiologie,

Königin-Luise-Strasse 28/30, 14195 Berlin, Germany

bjoern@brembs.net, http://brembs.net

Composite learning consists of two components with reciprocal, hierarchical interactions.

The AC-dependent classical or allocentric learning system inhibits the PKC-dependent operant or egocentric learning system via the mushroom-bodies. Operant behavior controlling predictive stimuli facilitates learning about these stimuli by the classical system via unknown, non-mushroom-body pathways. These interactions lead to efficient learning, generalization and prevent premature habit-formation.

Composite learning consists of two components with reciprocal, hierarchical interactions.

The AC-dependent classical or allocentric learning system inhibits the PKC-dependent operant or egocentric learning system via the mushroom-bodies. Operant behavior controlling predictive stimuli facilitates learning about these stimuli by the classical system via unknown, non-mushroom-body pathways. These interactions lead to efficient learning, generalization and prevent premature habit-formation.

classical/

allocentric

operant/

egocentric

Facili- tation

Inhi- bition

mb-dependent

rut-dependent

mb-independent

PKC-dependent

Composite learning

6. Conclusion

torque meter

yaw torque signal

diffusor light guides light source

IR laser diode

solenoid

color filter

classical component

operant component operant + classical

Composite training:

operant and classical components

Training Test

(1)

(2)

(3)

3. Learning-by-doing is most effective (in flies, too)

3. Learning-by-doing is most effective (in flies, too)

Fig. 3: Comparison of operant and classical pattern learning in flies.

The same sequence of sensory input sufficient for inducing a substantial learning effect if controlled operantly (left), only induces a small learning score if it is perceived passively (”classical”, right). Thus, active learning (”by doing”) is more effective than passive learning.

Left/red - Operant flies. N=30.

Right/blue - Classical flies. N=30.

Fig. 3: Comparison of operant and classical pattern learning in flies.

The same sequence of sensory input sufficient for inducing a substantial learning effect if controlled operantly (left), only induces a small learning score if it is perceived passively (”classical”, right). Thus, active learning (”by doing”) is more effective than passive learning.

Left/red - Operant flies. N=30.

Right/blue - Classical flies. N=30.

-0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

PI [r el. units]

30 30

operant classical

AC PKC

classical

operant both

5. Mushroom-bodies prevent premature habit formation

5. Mushroom-bodies prevent premature habit formation

PI [r el. units]

*

*

20 19

mb247 x TNT

-0.2

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

21

A

genetic controls

* B

*

27 32 15

17 17 20

* *

-0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4

0.6 C

17D x TNT 17 14

14

* *

(2) Isolated operant component

(1) composite control (operant + classical)

(3) Isolated classical component

WT - extended training

17 21

12

c205 x TNT

*

*

-0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

D

E

Fig. 5: The mushroom-body α

and β lobes but not the γ lobes

are necessary for inhibition of the operant component and ge- neralization of classical memory.

A. Flies with blocked MB output perform well in composite learning (red), but do not inhibit the operant component during com- posite training (green). Without inhibition of operant system, these transgenic flies are unable to generalize the isolated classi- cal component to a novel behavior (blue).

B. The genetic control flies (the two hete- rozygote strains did not differ and were pooled) reproduce the wild-type results:

significant composite learning, inhibition of the operant system and successful genera- lization of the isolated classical compo- nent. C. Flies with blocked output only from the α and β lobes of the MB mimic the flies expressing tetanus toxin in all MB lobes. They perform well in composite lear- ning, do not inhibit the operant system and do not generalize. D. Specificity of our mushroom-body effects is provided by expressing TNT in the fan-shaped body.

These flies behave as wildtype and control heterozygote flies with significant compo- site learning and inhibition of the operant system, which in turn allows for a success- ful generalization of the classical compo- nent to a novel behavior. E. Extended trai- ning in wildtype flies constitutes a pheno- copy of the transgenic animals (A). The longer training duration does not lead to an overtraining decrement. Testing for the operant component shows a release from the inhibition of operant learning. Without inhibition of the operant system, the flies are unable to generalize.

bjoern@brembs.net, http://brembs.net

WT

26

*

0.0 0.2 0.4

0.6 * *

noHS PKCi HS

PKCi rut

16 20 23

n.s.

AC PKC

PI [r el. units]

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

0 15 30 45 60 75 90 105 120

Time [s]

Yaw torque [arb. units]

-80 -40 0 40 80

0

Pattern position [°]

-180 -90 90

B

180

C

electric motor torque meter

yaw torque signal

diffusor light guides light source

IR laser diode

solenoid

color filter

1. Classical or

Pavlovian learning 1. Classical or

Pavlovian learning

Fig. 1: Manipulation of AC, but not of PKC disrupts learning of a classical predictor.

A – Experimental setup. The fly controls the angular position of a drum with four identical vertical bars in a flight simulator- like situation. The coloration of the arena is switched between bars, such that flying towards one pair of opposing bars leads to green coloration and towards the other pair to blue coloration. During training, heat is made contingent on one color, irre- spective of the turning maneuver which changed flight direction. B – Sample data from a wildtype fly during the first test period after the final training with heat on blue coloration. The fly uses both left and right turning maneuvers (red trace) to change flight direction (blue trace) and hence coloration of the environment (background color of the graph). The fly shows a clear preference for green with only brief excursions into flight directions which lead to blue color, even though the heat is switched off. C – Pooled performance indices (PI) from the first test period after training. In this and all subsequent bar graphs: Displayed are means, error bars are s.e.m. Numbers at bars – number of animals; rut – rut-mutant flies affecting AC; WT – wildtype; HS PKCi – heat shock induced expression of the specific PKC inhibitor; noHS PKCi – PKCi expression not induced; n.s. – not significant, * – p<0.05.

A

torque meter

yaw torque signal

diffusor light guides light source

IR laser diode

30

*

WT noHS

PKCi HS

PKCi

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

17

* *

rut

23

20

n.s.

AC PKC

PI [r el. units]

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

43 41

noHS het.c.

HS het.c.

*

*

45 60 75 90 105 120

Time [s]

C

Fig. 4: Manipulation of PKC but not of AC disrupts learning of a purely operant predictor.

A – Experimental setup. There are no visual cues for the fly. During training, heat is made contingent on either left- or right-turning yaw torque. B – Sample data from a wildtype fly during the first test period after the final training with heat on positive (right- turning) yaw torque. The fly only briefly generates right-turning yaw torque during the test phase (unsaturated red/blue bar under- neath dark red yaw torque trace), even though the heat is switched off. C – Pooled performance indices (PI) from the first test period after training. HS het.c. – Heat shock-treated heterozygous parental controls strain; noHS het.c. – Heterozygous parental control strain without heat shock.

4. Purely operant learning is different

40

Yaw torque [arb. units]

B

-40 0

0 15 30

A

bjoern@brembs.net, http://brembs.net

32

*

WT

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

17

* *

noHS PKCi HS

PKCi rut

27 21

n.s.

AC PKC

PI [r el. units]

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6

15 30 45 60 75 90 105 120

Time [s]

B

torque meter

yaw torque signal

diffusor light guides light source

IR laser diode

solenoid

color filter

2. Operant or

instrumental learning 2. Operant or

instrumental learning

Fig. 2: Operant learning requires the same gene as learning a classical predictor.

A – Experimental design. Throughout the experiment, one yaw torque domain is coupled to one color and the other to the other color (e.g., right turning causes green illumination and left turning blue illumination of the environment). During trai- ning, heat is made contingent on one of the two yaw torque/color combinations. B – Sample data from a wildtype fly during the first test period after the final training with heat on positive (right-turning) yaw torque (red trace) and blue illumination (background coloration). The fly shows the yaw torque domain/color preference and only briefly ventures into the previously punished situation, even though the heat is switched off. C – Pooled performance indices (PI) from the first test period after training.

C

-80 -40 0 40 80

Yaw torque [arb. units] 0

A

Referenzen

ÄHNLICHE DOKUMENTE

Canton S and genetic control lines perform well in both learning situations, whereas a FoxP mutant line and a FoxP RNAi line show significantly reduced learning scores specifically

Bacillus anthracis and Bordetella pertussis, the causative bacteria of anthrax disease and whooping cough, respectively, secrete the adenylyl cyclase (AC) toxins

These flies behave as wildtype and control heterozygote flies with significant compo- site learning and inhibition of the operant system, which in turn allows for a success- ful

As could be suspected from the large learning score with 2 minute periods, experiments with reduced period duration reveal that rut mutant flies indeed show enhanced operant

Because the mushroom- bodies are involved in some forms of generalization, I trained flies with blocked mushroom-body output in a situation with both operant and classical

These experiments show that in vivo and in vitro operant condi- tioning of Aplysia feeding behavior produce the same kind of neural correlates of the operant memory?. Thus, we

One day after implanting the electrodes, an- imals were assigned to one of three groups: (i) a control group without any stimulation, (ii) a contingent reinforcement group for

26: The mean polarity-latency indices for the operant test groups (A, dotted bars) indicate, that on average the first spikes away from the pattern are generated earlier in